Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
This is the CEC archive of Psyche through 2000. Psyche is now published by Hindawi Publishing.

Fred Punzo.
Diet and feeding behavior of the solpugid, Eremobates palpisetulosus (Solpugida: Eremobatidae).
Psyche 100(3-4):151-162, 1993.

This article at Hindawi Publishing: https://doi.org/10.1155/1993/13429
CEC's scan of this article: http://psyche.entclub.org/pdf/100/100-151.pdf, 740K
This landing page: http://psyche.entclub.org/100/100-151.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted automatically from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

DIET AND FEEDING BEHAVIOR OF THE SOLPUGID, EREMOBATES PALPISETULOSUS
(SOLPUGIDA: EREMOBATIDAE)
Dept. of Biology, Box 5 F
University of Tampa
Tampa, FL 33606
Eremobates palipsetulosus is a generalist predator which preys upon a wide variety of arthropods and seems to prefer smaller, soft-bodied prey items. Spiders as well as orthopteran, lepi- dopteran and coleopteran insects comprise 76% of the total diet. When a prey item is encountered, the palpi are used to pull the prey toward the chelicerae. The prey is then grasped in the che- licerae which are used to fragment and grind the prey tissues for subsequent ingestion. Increased hunger levels result in signifi- cantly faster ingestion time and higher capture rates. This is the first quantitative study on prey capture and feeding behavior for solpugids.
Solpugids represent an important component of the arachnid fauna inhabiting desert regions worldwide (Muma, 1967; Clouds- ley-Thompson, 1977). Although the predatory nature of these arachnids is well known (Bolwig, 1952; Muma, 1966; Wharton, 1987), no detailed analysis is available concerning the dietary pref- erences and feeding relationships of solpugids. Muma (1966) pro- vided some general information on ingestive behavior, search and location of potential prey, and reactions toward prey items offered in the laboratory, but no data are available concerning actual prey items captured in the field.
In this study, I examined the following parameters associated with feeding in Eremobates palpisetulosus Fichter: (1) prey species captured in the field; (2) dietary preferences based on prey Manuscript received 12 July 1993.
151




================================================================================

Psyche [Vol. 100
size and hardness of prey cuticle; (3) percent capture success as a function of hunger level (food deprivation); and (4) the effect of hunger level and predator size on ingestion time. MATERIALS AND METHODS
Eremubates palpisetulusus is a common inhabitant of the north- ern region of the Chihuahuan Desert (Muma, 1951). Adult males (18-23 mm, total body length), females (19-24 mm) and imma- tures (<lo mm) were collected while wandering the surface at night during May-August, 1991. Solpugids were collected within a 12-km radius of Terlingua, Texas (Brewster Co.), which lies within the Big Bend Region of Trans Pecos Texas. Two consecu- tive years of higher-than-normal rainfall was most likely an impor- tant factor contributing to the high numbers of solpugids encountered during this period. A detailed description of the geol- ogy and vegetational zones of this region is given by Tinkam (1948). A helmet-mounted light with a red filter was used to locate and observe solpugids as they moved across the substrate. Each animal was captured and examined for identification according to Muma (195 1) as well as for the presence of prey in the chelicerae. A total of 466 solpugids were observed, 92 of which (19.7%) pos- sessed a prey item. The prey from each solpugid was placed in alcohol for subsequent identification to order or family depending on the degree of digestion (Table 1).
Since adult females were encountered more frequently than males, subsequent laboratory studies were conducted on females. These solpugids were placed individually in plastic containers (20 X 15 X 6 cm), provided with water and fed on a diet of mealworm larvae (Tenebriu mulitor) and crickets (Acheta dumestica). These species were chosen because they are not found at the collection sites and therefore would minimize any choice bias as noted in confounding subsequent prey acceptability studies. Solpugids were maintained at 22' å 1å¡ and 70-80% relative humidity as described by Punzo (1993).
Twenty solpugids were selected at random for an analysis of dietary preferences as reflected by prey species actually captured and ingested by E. palpisetulusus. All prey species in these experi- ments were collected from the same location as the solpugids. The experimental protocol was identical to that described by Young



================================================================================

(1989) and Punzo (1991) for lycosid spiders. Solpugids were deprived of food for one week prior to testing. Individual solpugids were placed in a clean plastic container (15 X 10 X 5 cm) used as a test chamber. Prey organisms of different taxa and size classes were used (Table 2). An individual prey organism was placed in the center of the container with a solpugid and the status of the prey (rejected or captured and ingested) was recorded after 24 hr. Unconsumed material was removed after each test. Each prey species was offered to 20 different solpugids. In order to min- imize any effect of experience, prey of the same species were never presented consecutively as suggested by Nentwig (1987). All feeding trials were recorded using a Cine-8 high speed video cam- era (Visual Instrumentation Corp.) at 100 frameslsec. A Lafayette Super 8 Analyzer (Model 1026) was used for frame-by-frame analysis as described by Punzo (1989).
To determine the effects of hunger level on percent capture suc- cess (Table 3), 20 different solpugids were tested individually at each of four levels of food deprivation (6, 24, 48 and 72 hr) according to the method described by Punzo (1989). In order to standardize test trials, these experiments were conducted using a runway apparatus as described by Punzo (1989). The runway (15 X 8 X 6 cm) was constructed of clear plexiglass. The solpugid was placed in a holding chamber (9 X 10 X 6 cm) in a foreward-facing position at one end of the runway. The prey item to be tested (A. domestics) was placed in a compartment (5 X 8 X 6 cm) provided with a restraining door at the opposite end of the runway. Solpugids were allowed to habituate to the holding chamber for 30 min prior to the initiation of test trials. The degree of hunger (food deprivation) was defined as the time (hr) elapsed since the last feeding according to Dethier (1982). Before the start of each test trial, a cricket was placed in the prey compartment. A movable restraining door prevented the cricket from entering the runway. At the start of each trial, the restraining door was lifted manually and a gentle stream of compressed air was introduced through an intake valve leading to the back of the prey compartment. In response to the air flow, the cricket would immediately move directly into the runway and toward the solpugid positioned at the opposite end. The percent capture success was recorded for all trials. A capture attempt was considered to be successful if a solpugid firmly grasped a cricket in its chelicerae and began to



================================================================================

154 Psyche [vo~. 100
Table 1. Prey items captured by Eremobates palpisetulosus during field observa- tions (N = 92 solpugids and prey items). Life cycle stage of prey: A (adult), N (nymph), I (immature), L (larva). Percentage of total prey is given in parentheses. - - ----
Prey taxon
Number of prey items
Arachnida (1 5.2)
Araneae
Lycosidae
Lycosa avida (A)
Pardosa sternalis (A)
Undetermined (A)
Insecta
Coleoptera (1 5.2)
Carabidae (A)
Cicindelidae (A)
Scarabaeidae (A)
Tenebrionidae (A)
Undetermined (A)
Dictyoptera (2.2)
Blattellidae (A)
Diptera (9.7)
Asilidae (A)
Muscidae (A)
Tabanidae (A)
Tipulidae (A)
Undetermined (A)
Heteroptera (7.7)
Lygaeidae (N)
Miridae (A)
Pentatomidae (N)
Reduviidae (A)
Undetermined (N)




================================================================================

Punzo
Table 1 (continued)
Prey taxon
Number of prey items
Lepidoptera (1 3.1)
Geometridae (A)
Noctuidae (A)
Nymphalidae (A)
Undetermined (A)
(L)
Neuroptera (2.2)
Chrysopidae (A)
Orthoptera (32.6)
Acrididae
Bootettix sp. (N, A)
Brachystole sp. (N)
Schistocerca sp. (A)
Trimerotropis sp. (A)
Undetermined (N, A)
Gryllidae
Anabrus sp. (A)
Undetermined (N, A)
Tettigoniidae (A)
Undetermined (A)
Vertebrata (2.2)
Reptilia
Gekkonidae
Coleonyx brevis (I)




================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 100
Table 2. Laboratory experiments on prey items captured and ingested by Eremo- bates palpisetulosus adult females. Life cycle stage of prey: A (adult), N (nymph), I (immature). All prey items were collected from the same locality as E. palpisetulo- sus. N = 20 for each prey species.
Mean body
Mean body Percentage
length of
length of capture and
Prey species prey (mm) predator (mm) ingestion Araneae
Lycosidae
Lycosa avida (A)
(1)
Coleoptera
Carabidae
Calosoma scrutator (A)
Cicindela punctulata (A)
Meloidae
Lytta magister (A)
Silphidae
Silpha sp.
Tenebrionidae
Conibius
Dictyoptera
Blattellidae
Blattella
Hymenoptera
Formicidae
Pheidole
Mutillidae
Dasymutilla magnijica (A)
Orthoptera
Acrididae
Bootettix argentatus (A)
Schistocerca vaga (A)
(N)
Solpugida
E. palpisetulosus (A)
(1)




================================================================================

19931 Punzo 157
Table 3. Percent capture success for Eremobates palpisetulosus adult females (mean body length: 21.4 mm) as a function of food deprivation (in hr). N = 20 solpugids for each deprivation level. Prey species: Acheta domestics (mean body length: 10.7 mm).
--
Food deprivation (hr) Percent capture success (%) ingest the prey. An attempt was considered a failure if the solpugid missed the cricket entirely or if it grasped the prey only momentar- ily before it escaped.
Experiments were also conducted to assess the effects of hunger and predator size on the amount of time required to ingest a single cricket weighing 0.25 å .05 g. Two predator size classes (body length: 8.7-9.9 mm; weight 0.6 å .03 g; and 20.2-23.0 mm; 1.5 å .27 g), each comprising 15 different solpugids, were deprived of food for 6, 24, 48 or 72 hr prior to testing. The amount of time (min) required for each solpugid to ingest the prey was recorded. Ingestion time was defined as the amount of time that elapsed between initial grasping of the prey and the discarding of undi- gested cuticular fragments (Punzo, 1989). Each solpugid was tested only once in order to minimize the effect of experience. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Eremobates palpisetulosus captures a wide variety of prey (Table 1) and as such can be considered a generalist predator (Curio, 1976). Prey consisted of spiders, insects and lizards. Orthopteran insects comprised 32.6% of the total prey items, fol- lowed by spiders (1 5.2%), Coleoptera (1 5.2%), Lepidoptera (13.1%), Diptera (9.7%), Heteroptera (7.7%), Neuroptera (2.2%), Dictyoptera (2.2%), and geckonid lizards (2.2%). As a group, insects comprised 82.6% of the diet. Two solpugids were found holding the partially macerated bodies of the gecko, Coleonyx bre- vis, in their chelicerae. This nocturnal gecko is fairly common at this study area (Punzo, 1974) suggesting that E. palpisetulosus is



================================================================================

158 Psyche [vo~. 100
capable of capturing and ingesting small vertebrates. Preying on lizards agrees with the observations of Banta and Marer (1972) who reported a solpugid, Galeodes sp., feeding on a hatchling lizard. It is interesting to note that arthropods with well known chemical defenses such as blister beetles (Meloidae), velvet ants (Mutillidae), ants (Formicidae), scorpions and centipedes, all of which are common in this area and mostly nocturnal during sum- mer months (Punzo, 1989; Punzo and Huff, 1989), were not taken by these solpugids. Further evidence is provided by the laboratory experiments on prey acceptability (Table 2). The results from the laboratory experiments on prey capture are shown in Table 2. These observations support the field observa- tions. Smaller prey are captured and ingested with greater fre- quency than those whose body size closely approximates that of the solpugids. For example, the capture rate for blattellid nymphs is 85%, but only 60% for the larger adults (G = 11.9, p < 0.01, Sokal and Rohlf, 1981). The capture rate for nymphs of Schisto- cerca vaga (70%) is much higher than that for the adults (0%) (G = 19.7, p < 0.001). This is in contrast to results of similar experi- ments with spiders and scorpions, many of which frequently cap- ture and ingest prey items significantly larger than themselves (Curio, 1976; Nentwig, 1987; Punzo, 1989, 1991). In addition, hard-bodied prey such as the carabid beetles, Calosoma scrutator and Cicindela punctulata, and the tenebrionid beetle, Conibius gagates, are captured with relatively low success (0-25%). Well defended prey such as the blister beetle, Lytta magister, and the velvet ant, Dasymutilla magnifica, were never captured by these solpugids. Eremobates palpisetulosus exhibits a high capture rate for soft-bodied prey such as the silphid beetle, Silpha sp., imma- ture lycosid spiders, blattellid roaches, and acridid nymphs (60-85%). Note that immature conspecifics are also readily cap- tured and ingested although cannabilism was not observed in any of the field-collected solpugids. It may be that without the con- straints of the test chamber, solpugids, well known for their rapid and erratic locomotor movements (Turner, 19 16; Cloudsley- Thompson, 1961, 1977; Muma, 1967), may be too difficult to catch under natural conditions.
I want to point out that 86% of all solpugids encountered in the field were actively running across the surface in what seemed to be a random manner, rapidly tapping the ground in front of them with



================================================================================

19931 Punzo 159
the palpi. This type of random cursorial searching behavior has been reported for a few other solpugid species (Muma, 1966) with the exception of termitophilous species which appear to exhibit more sedentary habits (Cloudsley-Thompson, 196 1 ; Muma, 1967). In laboratory experiments, E. palpisetulosus exhibited well defined prey capture behavior as indicated by an analysis of video record- ings. These solpugids responded quickly to tactile stimuli and locomotor activity of prey upon contact with their palpi or other legs. They appear capable of orienting toward substrate vibrations but will attack only if actual physical contact with the prey is achieved. I have not seen any evidence for careful stalking of prey by E, palpisetulosus as reported for one ammotrechid solpugid (Pocock, 1898) and several eremobatid species (Muma, 1966). When prey is encountered by E. palpisetulosus, initial contact is made with the palpi which are then used to pull the prey toward the chelicerae. The prey is then grasped firmly with the chelicerae. This is followed by a vertical motion of the movable cheliceral fin- ger against the upper fondal teeth resulting in the fragmentation and grinding of prey tissues. In some cases, specific body parts of the prey such as the metathoracic legs (femurs) of acridids and gryllids, were severed and moved repeatedly through the cheliceral mill until most of the nonsclerotized tissue was ingested. Body parts such as wings, antennae, pro- and mesothoracic legs, and head capsules were frequently severed and discarded. In grinding the abdominal regions, especially in acridids, the intestinal tract was usually left intact and not ingested. The repeated grinding movements of the chelicerae were accompanied by a rhythmic pumping of the pharynx resulting in the transport of liquified food into the alimentary tract.
The ability of E. palpisetulosus to capture prey successfully increases significantly as a function of hunger level (Table 3). Solpugids deprived of food for 72 hr exhibited a successful capture rate of 55%. This dropped to 35% for those deprived of food for 48 hr (G = 18.3, p < 0.01; Sokal and Rohlf, 1981). There was also a significant difference between capture rates for solpugids deprived of food for 24 vs. 48 hr (G = 11.6, p < 0.01). There was no signifi- cant difference between animals deprived for 6 vs. 24 hr. This con- trasts sharply with the results obtained for the theraphosid spider, Dugesiella echina, from the same region, which exhibited a cap- ture rate of 91% when deprived of food for 72 hr (Punzo, 1989).



================================================================================

160 Psyche [VOI. 100
Data on additional species (Ford, 1978; Nentwig, 1987; Punzo, 1991) also indicate a more efficient prey capture rate for wander- ing spiders as compared to the solpugid tested in these experi- ments. One possible explanation for this may be that many wandering spiders move readily between patches, but once reach- ing a suitable patch they frequently adopt a sit-and-wait ambush strategy (Nentwig, 1987). This may increase their level of respon- siveness to potential prey that move into their awareness field (Curio, 1976) as compared to a predator that locates its prey pri- marily using vigorous, random and continued cursorial locomotor activity. Since these data are the first to be reported for a solpugid, it would be interesting to assess the capture rate for other solpugid species to see whether or not this difference is consistent across species.
The effects of food deprivation and predator size on ingestion time are shown in Table 4. A Model I1 ANOVA (Sokal and Rohlf, 1981) showed a significant overall effect of deprivation level (F = 151.4; df = 3, 44; p < 0.001) and predator size (F = 88.6; df = 1, 44; p < 0.001) on ingestion time. Scheffe F-tests (Sokal and Rohlf, 1981) showed a significant difference in ingestion time between predator size classes at all levels of deprivation (p < 0.01 for 6, 24 and 72 hr of deprivation; p < 0.05 for 48 hr). Larger solpugids exhibited faster ingestion rates at all hunger levels. Regardless of predator size, increased hunger resulted in faster ingestion of food. This suggests that ingestion rate is not solely determined by mechanical limitations imposed by morphological features of the mouth parts and accessory feeding structures. These experiments show that the level of food deprivation affects several parameters of feeding in E. palpisetulosus. Although hunger is generally associated with a collective series of internal messages related to caloric deficit (Dethier, 1982), it has proved difficult to observe and quantify (Punzo, 1989). The degree of hunger is influenced by the elapsed time between feeding bouts and has been shown to affect the degree of responsiveness of a predator toward potential prey (Curio, 1976). An increase in responsiveness to prey should lead to higher capture rates and faster ingestion time. The findings of this study support these predictions.




================================================================================

19931 Punzo 161
Table 4. The effect of food deprivation (hr) and predator size on the amount of time (min) required for Eremobates palpisetulosus females to ingest a single cricket weighing 0.3 å 0.05 g. Values represent means (å S.D.) for 15 solpugids in each size class.
Food deprivation
Mean ingestion time (min)
Predator size class (mean body length)
9.3 (0.6) mm 21.6 (1.4) mm
I thank James Bottrell and Thomas Punzo for assistance in the collection of solpugids in the field, Brian Garman, Dept. of Mathe- matics, for consultation on statistical analyses, Wayne Price for comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript, and Dan McCarty for permission to collect on private property. I also thank the Uni- versity of Tampa for a Faculty Development Grant which made much of this work possible and anonymous reviewers for com- ments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. LITERATURE CITED
BANTA, B. H., AND P. J. MARER
1972. An attack by a solpugid on an iguanid lizard hatching. Br. J. Herpetol. 4: 266.
BOLWIG, N.
1952. Observations on the behavior and mode of orientation of hunting Solifugae. J. Ent. SOC. S. Afr. 15: 239-240. CLOUDSLEY-THOMPSON. J. L.
Some aspects of the physiology and behavior of Galeodes arabs. Ent. exp. appl. 4: 257-263.
Adaptational biology of Solifugae (Solpugida). Bull. Br. arachnol. Soc. 4: 61-71.
The Ethology of Predation. Springer Verlag, New York. 274 pp. G.
The contribution of insects to the study of motivation. In: Changing Concepts of the Nervous System (A. Morrison and P. Strick, eds.). Aca- demic Press, New York. pp 445-455.




================================================================================

FORD, M. J.
1978. Locomotor activity and the predatory strategy of the wolf spider Par- dosa amentata (Clerck) (Lycosidae). Anim. Behav. 26: 31-35. MUMA, M. H.
195 1. The arachnid order Solpugida in the United States. Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 97: 34-141.
1966. Feeding behavior of North American Solpugida (Arachnida). Fla. Ent. 49: 199-216.
1967. Basic behavior of North American Solpugida. Fla. Ent. 50: 115-123. NENTWIG, W.
1987. The prey of spiders. In: Ecophysiology of Spiders (W. Nentwig, ed.), Springer Verlag, New York. pp. 249-263.
POCOCK, R. I.
1898. On nature and habits of Pliny's solpugs. Nature 57 (1487): 618-620. PUNZO, F.
1974. An analysis of the stomach contents of the gecko, Coleonyx brevis. Copeia 1974 (3): 779-780.
1989. Effects of hunger on prey capture and ingestion in Dugesiella echina Chamberlin (Orthognatha, Theraphosidae). Bull. Br. arachnol. Soc. 8: 72-79.
1991. Field and laboratory observations on prey items taken by the wolf spi- der, Lycosa lenta Hentz (Araneae, Lycosidae). Bull. Br. arachnol. Soc. 8: 261-264.
1993. Intraspecific variation in response to temperature and moisture in Ere- mobates palpisetulosus Fichter (Solpugida, Eremobatidae). Bull. Br. arachnol. Soc. (in press).
PUNZO, F., AND G. HUFF
1989. Comparative temperature and water relations and the effects of thermal acclimation on Tenebrio molitor and Tenebrio obscurus (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 93A: 527-533. SOKAL, R. R., AND F. J. ROHLF
1981. Biometry. 2nd ed., W. H. Freeman and Company, New York. 859 pp. TINKAM, E. R.
1948. Faunistic and ecological studies on the Orthoptera of the Big Bend Region of Trans Pecos Texas. Am. Midi. Nat. 40: 521-563. TURNER, C. H.
1916. Notes on the feeding behavior and oviposition of a captive American false-spider (Eremobates formicaria Koch). J. Anirn. Behav. 6: 160-168.
WHARTON, R. A.
1987. Biology of the diurnal Metasolpuga picta (Kraepelin) (Solifugae, Solipugidae) compared with that of nocturnal species. J. Arachnol. 14: 363-383.
YOUNG, 0. P.
1989. Predation by Pisaurina mira (Araneae, Pisauridae) on Lygus lineolaris (Heteroptera, Miradae) and other arthropods. J. Arachnol. 17: 43-48.



================================================================================


Volume 100 table of contents