Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
This is the CEC archive of Psyche through 2000. Psyche is now published by Hindawi Publishing.

Eric A. Greenquist and Jerome S. Rovner.
Lycosid Spiders on Artificial Foliage: Stratum Choice, Orientation Preferences, and Prey-Wrapping.
Psyche 83(2):196-209, 1976.

This article at Hindawi Publishing: https://doi.org/10.1155/1976/38620
CEC's scan of this article: http://psyche.entclub.org/pdf/83/83-196.pdf, 948K
This landing page: http://psyche.entclub.org/83/83-196.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted automatically from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

LYCOSID SPIDERS ON ARTIFICIAL FOLIAGE:
STRATUM CHOICE, ORIENTATION PREFERENCES, AND PREY-WRAPPING'
BY ERIC A. GREENQUIST AND JEROME S. ROVNER Department of Zoology, Ohio University, Athens, Ohio 45701 The majority of lycosid spiders neither spin webs for prey capture nor hunt actively, but spend most of their time waiting for prey at a resting site (Cragg, 1961; Edgar, 1969). In this regard, those lycosids that inhabit the herbaceous stratum resemble the aerial web-weaving spiders. As does a web, the foliage substratum provides a waiting site, a medium for transmission of vibratory stimuli produced by prey, and a surface on which capture is per- formed (Rovner and Knost, 1974). In the present study we used artificial foliage to examine stratum choice in Lycosa punctulata Hentz and L. rabida Walckenaer, which usually are found in the herbaceous stratum of fields, and in Schizocosa saltatrix (Hentz) and S. crassipes (Walckenaer), which typically are found on the leaf litter of forest floors. We also studied body orientation preferences shown by individuals of the first three species when they rested at an elevated site on the artificial foliage. The sig- nificance of a preference for vertical orientation was hypothesized with regards to equalizing proprioceptive input, minimizing energy expenditure, facilitating prey detection, and improving concealment from predators. Finally, we observed post-immo- bilization prey-wrapping to determine if this behavior is an adaptation for retention of captured prey by spiders dwelling in the herbaceous stratum, as hypothesized by Rovner and Knost (1974).
Sokal and Rohlf (1969) was the source of all parametric sta- tistical analyses. All means are accompanied by their standard deviation.
This study was supported in part by National Science Foundation Grant BMS- 7101589 to J. S. Rovner.
Manuscript received by the editor September 27, 1976



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 197 Spiders were collected in Athens Co., Ohio, USA, and main- tained under conditions described previously (Rovner and Knost, 1974). Adults of each species were examined during their natural season, i.e., when adults were abundant in the field. Studies were conducted in glass terraria (0.2 X 0.4 X 0.2 m high) contain- ing cardboard leaves with controlled surface areas and slopes. A 50-mm styrofoam layer on the bottom was covered with 5 mm of white sand, the latter being an inert substratum (Greenquist, 1975). Two cotton-stoppered vials provided drinking water. Cardboard plants, 0.18 m high and a mean width of 21 mm k 6.0 (range= 10-30 mm), were inserted into the styrofoam so that the tops reached within 10 mm of the glass lid. Three foliage designs were used (Fig. I):
Compound Design. This consisted of a vertical axis with one to four "leaf" projections coming off at 60' relative to the hori- zontal when viewed from an edge perspective. The surface area of the 6Q0 slopes equaled the surface area of the vertical blades. There were nine to eleven "plants" per terrarium. 60' / 90å Simple Design. Individual vertical and 60å blades such that the surface area of the vertical slopes equaled that of the 60' slopes; ten vertical and nine 60' blades per terrarium. 60' Simple Design. All blades positioned at 60'; eleven or twelve blades per terrarium. When viewed from a surface rather than an edge perspective, all the blades (or leaves) of all three designs pointed directly upward.
The relative humidity in the terraria averaged 94% k 3.8 and was maintained by a 250-ml bowl filled with cotton and distilled water. The high humidity minimized the possibility of a vertical gradient which otherwise might have influenced site selection, although Greenquist (1975) found that a difference of medium vs. high humidity levels (< 65% vs. > 85%) had no effect on stratum preference in L. punctulata. Temperature within the terraria averaged 26.7' k 4.5. The hygrometer and thermometer were housed in a terrarium identical to the test terraria but without foliage and spiders. Photoperiod was not controlled. Leaf litter was placed in the bottom of several terraria of the woodland- dwelling S. saltatrix to see if this influenced stratum preference. Individual experiments were conducted from 9 to 14 days with one spider ("solo") or three individually marked spiders of the



================================================================================

Psyche
[Jur
Fig. 1. Foliage designs used to study stratum choice and orientation preference in lycosid spiders. (Water bowls and vials are not shown.) (A) Compound Desigr (B) 60å¡/90 Simple Design. (C) 60' Simple Design.



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 199 same sex ("trio") in each terrarium. We observed the animals four times daily for 6 days at 3-hour intervals between 0900-1800. They were fed on the seventh day, and observations resumed on the eighth day. At each observation, the spider's height (from the center of the cephalothorax to the terrarium base), the slope of the resting site, and the orientation of the body relative to the blade or leaf axis were recorded. When cannibalism or death occurred (eleven cases) during trio experiments, replacement was made with equivalently marked animals. Five unmated females built egg sacs during the experiments; however, this did not in- fluence stratum preference (paired-comparisons test with arcsine transformation; variation due to egg sac, Fsn,4) = 0.323 NS; vari- ation among individuals, Ffl.4) = 1.892 NS). Stratum choice. - The differences in time spent on foliage vs. on the ground were significant among the four lycosid species (one-way ANOVA with arcsine transformation; Fso.56) = 3 1.189, P < 0.001). L. punctulata (12 females, 4 males) averaged 24.4% of the recorded position on the foliage (n = 832); L. rabida (5 fe- males, 13 males) averaged 41.1% (n = 989); S. crassipes ( 3 females, 15 males) averaged 1.2% (n = 595); and S. saltatrix (13 females, 7 males) averaged 9.1% (n = 1022). An a posteriori STP-test re- vealed no significant difference in preference between the two Lycosa spp. S. saltatrix had significantly greater herbaceous pref- erence than S. crassipes (P < 0.05) and significantly less than the Lycosa spp. (P < 0.001). S. crassipes is omitted from the follow- ing analyses due to its very low tendency to rest on the foliage. Stratum choice by L. punctulata and S. saltatrix was not af- fected by variation in foliage design or animal density; however, the tendency to spend time on the foliage in L. rabida, while not affected by foliage design, was significantly greater at solo density (Table I). Data on the individuals of these three species are avail- able in Greenquist (1975).
The presence of leaf litter in some of the terraria of S.saltatrix did not affect stratum preference for any foliage design (paired- comparisons test; Litter variation, Fsd.2) = 1.370 NS; Foliage vari- ation, FW) = 1.142 NS). This suggested that these spiders (and probably S. crassipes) would not seek the artificial foliage when



================================================================================

200 Psjxhe [June
Table I. Effect of spider density and foliage design on strati'in choice in three lycosids. Individuals were grouped according to density and foliage design. An arcsine transformation was performed on the % of readings in which each spider was on the foliage. The mean % of each group was analyzed with two-way ANOVA without replication for solo vs. trio densities and for Compound vs. 60å¡/90 Simple vs. 60' Simple foliage designs.
Species Source of variation df Mean square 5
Density 1 94.72 1.52 NS
Lycosa punctulata Foliage design 2 19.50 0.31 NS Error 2 62.27
Density 1 570.38 48.76*
Lycosa rabida Foliage design 2 23.62 2.02 NS Error 2 11.70
Density 1 0.001 0.00 NS
Schizocosa saltatrix Foliage design 2 7.07 0.15 NS Error 2 46.03
the leaf litter, characteristic of their natural habitat, was not avail- able in the experimental terraria.
Orientation preferences. - Leaf slope preference was analyzed by chi-square; we assumed that random preference would result in an equal number of positions recorded on vertical and 60' slopes. L. punctulata had a significant preference for the 60å slope over the vertical slope on the compound foliage design; L. rabida and S. saltatrix showed no preference on this foliage design (Table 11). L. punctulata was found on the verticalslope more often than on the 60' slope when housed with the 60å¡/90 Simple Design. L. rabida and S. saltatrix preferred the 60å slope in this case. We assumed that random selection of upper vs. lower surfaces on the 60' slopes would result in an equal number of positions recorded for each surface. Chi-square analysis indicated that all three species showed a significant preference for the upper sur- face on all three foliage designs (Table 11). The orientation of the spider within the plane parallel to the surface on which it rested was recorded as the angular difference between the blade (or leaf) axis and the spider's body axis. This



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 201 4
å ´ / I -
Z?Z
vvv
a.ii.4
* * *
* * *




================================================================================

202 Psyche [June
was measured clockwise to the nearest 30' interval, with O0 = facing directly upward (Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA was used to deter- mine angular preferences within species and differences among species. There was a significant difference in preference within species (Fs(l 1.22) = 13.943, P < 0.001), indicating that individuals had a preferred orientation on the artificial leaf. There was no difference, however, in angular preference among species (^(2,22) = 0.752 NS). Orientation to 180' (= facing directly downward) was significantly greater for the three species than to all other angles (a posteriori STP-test. P< 0.001). Orientation to 0' (di- rectly upward) was significantly greater than to all other angles except 180' (P < 0.01). Orientation to 180' was significantly greater than to 0' (P < 0.05). Spiders were not found to orient differently on the 60' and vertical slopes (paired-comparisons test; L. punctulata, FS(1.li) = 2.560 NS; L. rabida, F,T(~,~ 1) = 1 .Ol6 NS; S. saltatrix, Fd.11) = 1.194 NS).
ORIENTATION ( DEGREES CLOCKWISE FROM O0 1 Fig. 2.
Body orientation when resting on artificial foliage in Lycosa punctulata (obliquely hatched bar) (n = 203), L. rabida (solid bar) (n = 399), and Schizocosa saltatrix (horizontally hatched bar) (n = 93). O0 = facing directly upward. Since the predominant orientation in the three species was vertical (0' and 180å¡) there was the possibility of the data being biased due to the spider's grasping the edges of the vertically positioned leaves with its right or left tarsal claws. Therefore, we conducted additional experiments in four other terraria, using female L. rabida. Each of these terraria was subdivided into five,



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 203 equal-size compartments by parallel cardboard sheets placed ver- tically (two terraria) or at an oblique (60') angle (two terraria). These sheets extended the full width and height of the terraria, and were held in place by strips of masking tape along the side and bottom edges. Thus, each of the twenty spiders lived in a compartment with a narrow horizontal floor, two tall broad card- board walls, and narrow glass sides and top. (These lycosids can- not climb glass.) Observations were made on 11 days, four times per day, as above.
Under these conditions, the spiders were recorded on the card- board walls in 91.9% of the 834 total positions recorded; i.e., they usually were at an elevated location. In the oblique wall condi- tion, 95.5% of the 448 positions were on the wall providing an upper surface; i.e., the spider rarely adopted a position on the undersurface of the overhanging wall.
Of the 750 positions recorded on walls, under both vertical and oblique wall conditions, 21.7% were noted in which the spider held onto the top edge (the only available edge) of the cardboard. Most of these (69.3%) involved a 90' or 270å body orientation, since the edge-holding spider typically used the tarsal claws of the four legs of the right or left side to secure the hold. Most (78.3%) of the wall positions adopted were on the broad surface. Here, in the absence of an edge cue, 43.4% of the posi- tions were 0' (22.3%) or 180' (21. I%), i.e., facing directly up or down. The next most common orientation on the broad surface (18.6%) was with the body parallel to the ground, i.e., at 90' or 270'. No significant differences between the orientation prefer- ences of the spiders on these broad surfaces and the spiders on the artificial foliage were found (paired-comparisons test with arcsine transformation; FYu., = 1 .Ol4 NS). Methods. - Prey-wrapping behavior was examined in the artificial foliage terraria after prey were withheld from the spiders for up to 8 days. Prey items that either singly or in multiples of three or four would approximate the size of the spider's body were chosen to maximize prey-wrapping in the Lycosa spp. (Rovner and Knost, 1974). L. punctulata were given crickets (Gryllus sp. and Nemobiinae); L. rabida were fed grasshoppers (Cyrtacantha- cridinae). On several occasions, when prey-wrapping occurred



================================================================================

204 Psyche [June
on the foliage, the spider was chased from the prey with a soft brush to determine if the prey item was attached to the substrate and if the spider returned to the prey. S. crassipes and S. saltatrix were fed plant bugs (Miridae), thirty to forty bugs being placed in each terrarium. Multiple captures of up to six prey occurred, which should have stimulated prey-wrapping (ibid.). Sod (with vegetation intact) from the habitat of L. rabida was placed in a 0.5 X 0.26 X 0.3 m high glass terrarium and brought into the laboratory. After prey were captured in the home cage of an individual of this species, the spider was coaxed onto the grass of the terrarium, where it came to rest, still holding its prey. Observation periods for prey-wrapping studies lasted 2 hr. Results. - Prey transport was common in all four species and usually occurred before the prey were immobile. Immobile prey occasionally were transported from the ground into the foliage by all species except S. crassipes. Spiders transported prey with the chelicerae and walked with the body raised up; nevertheless, large prey often dragged on the ground.
None of the species used silk to immobilize the, prey. In the two Lycosa spp., females exhibited from one to five separate bouts (and males only one bout) of post-immobilization wrapping dur- ing the 2-hr observation period in response to single large prey and multiple captures of small prey. While L. punctulata wrapped in a counter-clockwise direction, L. rabida wrapped 65.3% of the time in a clockwise direction. L. rabida held prey in the chelicerae during the first wrapping bout for an average of 1.6 k 1.21 revolu- tions. Consequently, as the spider wrapped, the prey beneath it pivoted around with the spider. The spider then released the prey and continued to pivot above it for the rest of the bout. Hold- ing the prey in this manner while wrapping rarely occurred during subsequent bouts, only appearing if those bouts were preceded by excessive prey manipulation with the palps, and never lasted as long as it had in the original bout. L. punctulata never held prey in this manner while wrapping. Other details of wrapping resembled the description of Rovner and Knost (1974) and are provided in Greenquist (1975). Wrapping never occurred in S. crassipes (four females, nine males) nor S. saltatrix (fifteen females, nine males).
L. rabida was examined under the semi-natural conditions of a terrarium containing field sod. Here, with the spiders holding



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 205 onto blades of tall grass- prey-wrapping was slower than it had been on the broader, flat surfaces of the artificial foliage terraria (one-tailed t-test; tw = 2.922, P < 0.01). When the spiders were driven 90 mm or more from the feeding site (with a soft brush), they returned after as much as 2.5 min later in four out of six cases, and resumed feeding on the prey package still attached to the grass. On two occasions they came back by a different route than the one by which they had left. On another occasion a spider carried wrapped prey for 33 mm, dropped it (when touched by the brush), and continued on for another 85 mm. After 72 sec the spider re- turned to the drop site, although it id not find the prey, which had fallen to a point 30 mm below.
Stratum choice. -The selective advantage for any species to carry on a major portion of its activity in a specific micro-habitat is that this prevents interspecific competition. Kuenzler (1958) found that vertical stratification separates L. rabida from the ground-dwelling lycosids. The same would be true later in the year when L. punctulata replaces L. rabida in the herbaceous stratum. The results of our laboratory study support the idea of differential use of two strata by lycosids. The herbaceous stratum-dwelling Lycosa spp. spend significantly more time rest- ing on the artificial foliage than the two Schizocosa spp., which are found in nature on woodland floors.
While overall habitat selection was found to depend on the ability of various lycosid species to withstand desiccation (Cher- rett, 1964), the importance of the physical form of features within a lycosid's habitat in determining their micro-habitat distribution was emphasized by the field studies of Duffey (1962, 1966) and the work of Richter (1970). We found that our lycosids spend dissimilar amounts of time, depending on species, in different strata under conditions of uniformly high relative humidity and minimal (if any) temperature and light gradients. Thus, our data provide laboratory support for the idea that a preference for structural features within the habitat can play an important role in the micro-habitat distribution of wandering spiders. In our experimental terraria (Fig. 1) foliage design had no effect on stratum choice (Table I). Density (solo vs. trio) had an effect only in L. rabida, the reason for which we do not know. (Unlike



================================================================================

206 Psyche [June
L. punctulata, most of our L. rabida were males, which may tend to wander more and to interact agonistically at trio density, thereby spending less time resting at an elevated foliage site. This is only a guess.)
Orientation preferences. - Leaf slope preferences on the arti- ficial foliage were not consistent among and even within the species, some spiders being located most often on a 60' slope, others most often on a vertical surface (Table 11). These data, as well as the readiness of control spiders to rest on both 60å and vertical broad surfaces, suggest that there possibly is no prefer- ence for oblique vs. vertical leaves as resting sites in these lycosids. There was a definite preference, however, for upper- rather than undersurfaces of sloping (60') leaves (Table 11). Similarly, control spiders on the broad surfaces rarely rested while holding onto the undersurface of a 60' cardboard wall. An inverted posi- tion likely provides less secure "footholds" for resting or for chasing prey in these webless spiders. It also is less suitable for the critical act of pouncing on the prey. since the spider would have gravity working against it.
Most aerial web-weaving spiders orient in a face-down position while resting in their webs (Eberhard, 1967). Orientation prefer- ences also were shown by our lycosids on sloping or vertical sur- faces, the spider most often adopting a vertical position. As with web weavers, a vertical position possibly yields optimum "claw- holds" with the least amount of energy expenditure while waiting for prey. On artificial foliage, the most common resting position for lycosids, like that of web weavers, was facing directly down- ward. On a broad surface, the vertical orientation was again predominant, although the lycosids here faced up as often as down. Either way, equivalent input from the proprioceptors of the right and left sides is achieved, which may be the primary determinant of this orientation.
Visual and mechanical prey-detection by wandering spiders resting in the herbaceous stratum probably are enhanced by ver- tical orientation on stems or grass blades, since this aims the spider in one of the two main directions from which crawling prey are most likely to approach. Kuenzler (1958) noted that crawling prey are the primary food of lycosids.
A preferred vertical orientation on grasses and other plants may have influenced selection for the bold stripes running length-



================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 207 wise on the two Lj9cosa spp. found in this stratum, since such markings would provide cryptic coloration. Ground-dwelling lycosids lack such bold, anterior to posterior, striped patterns. The next most common orientation on artificial foliage and on broad surfaces was perpendicular to the vertical one, i.e.. 90å or 270' (when O0 =facing up). In such a horizontal position on a sloping or vertical surface, the spiders were holding primarily by the tarsal claws of the upper four ipsilateral legs. For some reason, positions intermediate between vertical and horizontal are less satisfactory for long-term resting postures on elevated surfaces. It may relate to the effective use of the inwardly curving tarsal claws for securing a grip or to a tendency to prefer similar proprioceptive inputs from four ipsilateral legs as the next best condition to that provided by the bilaterally symmetrical input of a vertical orientation. All things considered, it is likely that vertical orientation would pre- dominate in the field, where stems and the edges of grass blades provide largely vertical grasping sites for the claws that would add to the laboratory-demonstrated preference for a vertical posi- tion.
Prey-wrapping. - Our data supported Rovner and Knost's (1974) hypothesis that post-immobilization wrapping of prey by wandering spiders is an adaptation for successful feeding in the herbaceous stratum. This behavior did not occur in our ground- dwelling Schizocosa spp. Wrapping prevents prey loss from elevated sites when the cheliceral grip is relaxed during feeding, grooming, or a startle response, since wrapping always includes attachment of the prey to the site. Furthermore, when we forced spiders to leave immobilized prey, they were able to return to the site. Ob- viously, wrapped (i.e., attached) prey are far more likely to be recovered at an elevated site than are non-wrapped. Since the spiders sometimes returned by a route different from that taken when chased away, they were not depending on draglines to re- locate the prey. They may have used kinesthetic orientation, or visual orientation, or both (Gorner, 1966). Rovner and Knost (1974) also suggested that wrapping by lyco- sids might serve to free the spider for subsequent attacks on addi- tional prey, as occurs in web weavers. Out data did not support this idea. Subsequent captures were never observed in which the spider returned to the original prey at a previous site. as was also noted to be the case in Cupiennius salei (Melchers, 1963). Ap-



================================================================================

Psyche
[June
parently, in contrast to web-dwellers, which are likely to detect a prey while still handling a previous one (Eberhard, 1967) and which spend prolonged periods at fixed sites (in their webs), the retention of prey at a site for future use after additional captures is of little value to a wandering spider. When housed in terraria, Lycosa punctulata and L. rabida spend significantly more time on artificial foliage than do Schizo- cosa crassipes and S. saltatrix. Such data provide laboratory support for the idea that a preference for structural features within the habitat plays a role in the micro-habitat distribution of wan- dering spiders. Our lycosid species prefer upper rather than lower surfaces as rest sites. and we suggest that upper surfaces are co- incidentally more suitable for prey capture. On broad, steep sur- faces our species most often adopt a vertical orientation, facing directly up or down. On artificial foliage, vertical orientation likewise predominates, with a significant preference here for a face-down position, i.e., the position also typical of web-weaving spiders. We suggest mechanisms, one of which may underlie this orientation preference. Coincidentally, prey detection by wander- ing spiders that are at rest in the herbaceous stratum may be facilitated by a vertical orientation. Our observations on post- immobilization prey-wrapping indicate that wrapping not only enables wandering spiders to retain prey while feeding at an ele- vated site, but also insures the recovery of immobilized prey when the spider momentarily flees and then returns to the feeding site. CHERRETT, J. M.
1964. The distribution of spiders on the Moor House National Nature Reserve, Westmorland. J. Anim. Ecol. 33: 27-48.
CRAGG, J. B.
1961. Some aspects of the ecology of moorland animals. J. Anirn. Ecol. 30: 205-234.
DUFFEY, E.
1962. A population study of spiders in limestone grassland. The field-layer fauna. Oikos 13: 15-34.
1966. Spider ecology and habitat structure. (Arach., Araneae). Senck. biol. 47: 45-49.




================================================================================

19761 Greenquist & Rovner - Spiders on Artificial Foliage 209 EBERHARD, W.
1967. Attack behavior of diguetid spiders and the origin of prey wrapping in spiders. Psyche 74: 173-1 8 1.
EDGAR, W. D.
1969. Prey and predators of the wolf spider bcosa lugubris. J. Zool., Lond. 159: 4054 1 1.
CORNER, P.
1966. ~ber die Koppelung der optischen und kinasthetischen Orientierung bei den Trichterspinnen Agelena labyrinthica (Clerck) und Agelena gracilens C. L. Koch. Z. vergl. Physiol. 53: 253-276. GREENQUIST, E.
1975. Stratum preference and prey-wrapping behavior in four species of lycosid spiders. M.S. Thesis, Ohio University.
KUENZLER, E. J.
1958. Niche relations of three species of lycosid spiders. Ecology 39: 494-500. MEWHERS, M.
1963. Zur Biologie und zum Verhalten von Cupiennius salei (Keyserling), einer amerikanischen Ctenide. 2001. Jahrb. Abt. System. 91: 1-90. RICHTER, C. J. J.
1970. Relation between habitat structure and development of the glandulae ampullaceae in eight wolf spider species (Pardosa, Araneae. Lycosidae). Oecologia (Berl.) 5: 185-199.
ROVNER, J. S. AND S. J. KNOST
1974. Post-immobilization wrapping of prey by lycosid spiders .of the herba- ceous stratum. Psyche 81: 398415.
SOKAL, R. R. AND F. J. ROHLF
1969. Biometry. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. 776 pp.



================================================================================


Volume 83 table of contents