Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
This is the CEC archive of Psyche through 2000. Psyche is now published by Hindawi Publishing.

Todd E. Shelly.
Prey selection by the neotropical spider Alpaida tuonado with notes on web-site tenacity.
Psyche 90(1-2):123-133, 1983.

This article at Hindawi Publishing: https://doi.org/10.1155/1983/59379
This article at Biodiversity Heritage Library: https://www.biodiversitylibrary.org/page/50929901
CEC's scan of this article: http://psyche.entclub.org/pdf/90/90-123.pdf, 636K
This landing page: http://psyche.entclub.org/90/90-123.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted automatically from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

PREY SELECTION BY THE NEOTROPICAL SPIDER ALPAIDA TUONABO
WITH NOTES ON WEB-SITE TENACITY'
BY TODD E. SHELLY
Department of Biology
University of California
Los Angeles, CA 90024
Prey selection by web-building spiders includes 2 principle com- ponents. First, webs may catch a nonrandom sample of the avail- able prey (passive selection). Among items caught in the web, the spider may then feed on preferred prey but reject unsuitable prey (active selection). As evident from a recent review (Riechert and Luczak 19821, quantitative field measurements of either component are relatively rare and particularly so for tropical species. Here 1 compare the web contents of Alpaida ruonabo (Chamber- lin and hie) with sticky trap samples of available prey. Field work was conducted at one site over a relatively short period of time thus reducing potential complications arising from habitat and seasonal differences in prey availability. As Olive (1980) and Uetz et al. (1978) found, however, prey availability may vary over short verti- cal distances, and to examine this possibility potential prey were sampled at several different heights.
In addition, a second comparison was made between captured items being eaten and those left unattacked and uneaten. Since prey ignored during the day may have been consumed at night with the web, uneaten prey did not necessarily represent rejected prey. This comparison, however, does quantify the probability of immediate attack upon different types and sizes of captured prey. 'While Araneus is the accepted generic designation, this species is not closely related to other members of this genus and should perhaps be placed in the genus Aplaida (H. Levi pen. comm.).
Manuscript received by the editor December 20, 1982.



================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 90
The study was conducted between July 23 and August 25, 1980, on Barro Colorado Island (BCI), Panama. This time period falls near the middle of a rainy season which annually extends from late April to mid-December (Croat 1978). The island is covered by a lowland tropical moist forest (Holdridge et al. 1971). Alpaida tuo- nabo was most abundant on the island's central plateau, and all work was conducted there.
Very little is known about the biology of A. tuonabo. A descrip- tion of the female has been published (Chamberlin and Ivie 1936), but males have not yet been described (H. Levi, pers. comm.). Females are relatively small; the mean wet weight and body length of 8 adult females were 0.023 g (SD 0.005) and 5.6 mm (SD 0.94), respectively. Females appeared to construct and tend webs during the day and consume them at night. In 4 nights of searching, I never saw a female or an intact web. On BCI A. tuonabo is abundant only in the mid to late wet season (July to December) and is rarely found during the rest of the year (Lubin 1978). Flying insects were sampled at 10 different sites. At each site I implanted a 2.7 m PVC pole (diameter 25 mm) by driving 0.30 m - 0.45 m of its length into the ground. Wooden rods (length 30 mm; diameter 5 mm) were then fastened to the pole at 0.3 m intervals (from 0.3 m to 2.1 m above ground). Fastened at one end, each rod projected perpendicularly from the vertical pole and hence was parallel to the ground's surface. Insects were collected on tanglefoot covered traps suspended from the wooden rods. Each trap was a 15 cm by 23 cm rectangle of 3 mm thick transparent plastic coated on both sides with tanglefoot. Insects were sampled during the day only on August 7-9. Each day the traps were set between 0800 hrs-0900 hrs, taken down between 1600 hrs-1700 hrs, and stored overnight in closed boxes. Aside from Diptera and Hymenoptera, all trapped insects were identified to order. Flies were categorized as either nematocerous or non-nematocerous, and hymenopterans were subdivided into bees and wasps, parasitoids, and winged ants. All trapped insects were measured to the nearest 0.1 mm using a dissecting microscope equipped with a disc micrometer. Each day of the study I walked through different areas of the forest (between 0900-1630 hrs) and examined every web encoun- tered. All caught items were collected and labelled as either eaten or



================================================================================

19831 Shelly - Alpaida tuonabo
Diptera Hymenoptera Coleoptera
nematocerous ants
non-nematocerous ~arasitoids
Frequency
Figure 1. Vertical distributions of the major prey categories. Each value repres- ents the total number of individuals captured on 10 sticky traps suspended at a particular height. See text for details of sampling method. uneaten. Uneaten prey were also examined for evidence of wrap- ping. For each web thus sampled, the height of the spider was also recorded. Collected prey were later assigned to the appropriate prey category and measured to the nearest 0.1 mm. Prey selectivity was quantified using Ivlev's (1 96 1) index of elec- tivity. Electivity (E) is calculated as follows: E = (ri - pi)/(ri + pi) where ri is the proportion of the predator's diet represented by prey type (or size class) i, and pi is the proportion of the available prey represented by prey type (or size class) i. Values of E range from -1.0 (complete avoidance) to +1.0 (complete preference). In this study electivity values with absolute values less than 0.40 were not considered to differ significantly from zero. In addition, two sets of electivity values were calculated. For web selectivity (Ew) ri is the proportion of the web contents (both eaten and uneaten items) represented by prey type i, and pi is the proportion of available prey (as measured by the sticky traps) represented by prey type i. For spider selectivity (Es) ri is the proportion of the spider's observed diet (the eaten prey) represented by prey type i, and pi is the propor- tion of the web contents (both eaten and uneaten items) represented by prey type i.
Alpaida tuonabo females generally constructed webs in relatively open sections of the forest or at the edges of tree-fall gaps. Most web



================================================================================

126 Psyche [VOI. 90
sites were shaded, and only rarely was a web placed in an area that received direct sunlight. Various web support structures were util- ized, including leaf tips, herbaceous stems, woody vines and branches, and palm fronds. The circular webs averaged 21.6 cm in diameter and 350 cm* in catching area (n = 8). Individuals do not appear to remain at a particular web-site for more than 1-2 days. On August 3 1 marked the location of 20 occupied webs. These sites were then revisited daily for 7 days, and the presence or absence of the spider and the web was recorded. In terms of the number of spiders remaining at their initial site, the results obtained were as follows: Day 1-12; Day 2-3; Days 3 and 4-2; Days 5 and 6-1 ; Day 7-0. In no instance was a spider absent but the web present; spider and web were always both present or both absent. In addition, in examining a 2 m-3 m radius about each vacated web-site, 1 never observed the presence of a newly con- structed web.
Five prey categories comprised 89.0% of the total sample, and vertical abundance patterns were examined for these groups only. Beetles, parasitoid Hymenoptera, nematocerous and non- nematocerous Diptera all exhibited a similar trend in vertical abun- dance (Figure 1). That is, the greatest numbers of individuals were collected at the two lowest sampling heights (0.3 m and 0.6 m). While similar numbers of parasitoid Hymenoptera were captured at the two lowest sampling heights, nearly twice as many beetles, nema- tocerous and non-nematocerous Diptera were captured at 0.3 m than 0.6 m. Ants were captured in relatively constant numbers over all sampling heights.
Although the numbers of trapped individuals varied greatly with height for 4 prey categories, each major category comprised a rela- tively constant proportion of the total sample at each height (Figure 2). Similarly, within each category size frequency distributions did not vary with height in any obvious manner (Figure 3). Thus, while the abundance of flying insects varied with height, the taxonomic and size composition of this fauna did not. The vertical distribution of A. tuonabo did not closely match that observed for available prey (Figure 4). Alpaida tuonabo preferred web-sites between 0.6 m-1.2 m, and approximately 60% of the spi- ders measured were within this range. Thus, while traps nearest the ground caught the greatest numbers of flying insects, only 18% of A, tuonabo were found below 0.6 m.




================================================================================

19x31 Shelly - Alpaida tuonabo
Diptera
0 nematocerous
o non-nematocerobs
+
I
0.3
Hymenoptera
ants
Coleoptera
20 40 60
Number
Figure 2.
Relative abundances of major prey categories over all heights sam -r> 9. ed. Each value represents a proportion of the total number of individuals captured o n 10
sticky traps suspended at a particular height. See text for details of sampling met: l-Ìö ad. Non-nematocerous
Diptera
Ants
Nematocerous Parasitoid
Diptera Hymenoptera
Coleoptera
I f i f i l l
50 100 50 100
Frequency
Figure 3. Size frequency distributions for the major prey categories over t: b e 7
heights sampled. Within a category each value represents the proportion of ind å v -M du- als captured at a particular height that fell within a particular 1 mm interval - T h e symbols used for the various size classes are: 0 - I mm (0). 1 - 2 mm (0)- ZZ - 3 mm (X), and >3 mm (A).




================================================================================

128 Psyche [VOI. 90
A total of 446 insects representing 6 orders were taken from 320 webs of A. tuonabo. Approximately 95% of these insects belonged to those 5 prey categories which were most abundant in the sticky trap samples. Consequently, analysis of both web and spider selec- tivities will focus only upon these groups. In addition, since the composition of the flying insect fauna did not much vary with height, both the data regarding prey availability and diet were com- bined over all heights.
Web selectivity values did not differ greatly from zero for beetles, nematocerous Diptera, or parasitoid Hymenoptera (Table 1 ). Non- nematocerous Diptera, however, comprised a small proportion of the web contents relative to their proportion on the traps. Con- versely, ants represented a large proportion of the web contents relative to their proportion on the traps. Aside from nematocerous Diptera, A. tuonabo were observed to consume prey types in proportions roughly equal to their propor- tion in the web (Table 2). Spider selectivity values for beetles, ants, non-nematocerous Diptera, and parasitoid Hymenoptera were all less than 0.20 (absolute value). In contrast, the Es value for nema- tocerous Diptera was large and negative. A. tuonabo
o sticky traps
Number
Figure 4.
Vertical distribution of A. luonubo and available prey. Heights of hub-resting spiders were measured to the nearest cm and then placed into 0.3 m intervals. Values for prey represent the total number of insects captured on 10 sticky traps suspended at a particular height. See text for details of sampling method.



================================================================================

19831 Shelly - A/pale/a tuonaho 129
Table 1. Web selectivity (Ew) values for prey types collected from webs of A. ~uunabo.
Prey type
Collected from webs
(eaten and uneaten)
no.
i
Captured on traps
no. pi Ew
Beetles 56 12.5
Nematocerous Diptera 1 64 36.8
Non-nematocerous Diptera 34 7.6
Ants
128 28.7
Parasitoid Hymenoptera 42 9.4
Others
22* 4.8
*Others include: butterflies (6). bees and wasps (10). leafhoppers (4). thrips (2) **Others include: butterflies (2). bees and wasps (2). leafhoppers (80), thrips (27). Hemiptera (8). Orthoptera (5). Collembola (3). Zoraptera (4). Plecoptera (3). Isoptera (2 1 ), Psocoptera (20)
As the Es values imply, the majority (87%) of uneaten prey were nematocerous Diptera. Most of these, in turn, did not appear to have been wrapped. Many, in fact, were observed struggling in web while stuck by a single wing. Similarly, most uneaten non- nematocerous Diptera and parasitoid Hymenoptera were appar- ently unwrapped. In contrast, 9 of the 12 uneaten ants had clearly been attacked and wrapped.
Only 2 groups, nematocerous Diptera and ants, were found in webs in sufficient numbers to allow meaningful calculation of web selectivity values for different size classes. Nematocerans less than 1 mm were relatively more abundant in webs than on the traps, while the opposite was true for those between 1 mm-2 mm (Table 3a). Web selectivity values, however, did not differ greatly from zero for either size class. Ants in webs were rather uniformly distributed among 8 size classes (Table 3b). The majority (76.0%) of ants on the sticky traps, however, were less than 3 mm long. Consequently, web selectivity values for the 1 mm-2 mm and 2 mm-3 mm size classes were large and negative, while those for larger classes were all large and positive.
Only ants were eaten in sufficient numbers to allow meaningful calculation of spider selectivity values for different size classes. Yet, since nearly all (90.6%) of the ants taken from webs were being eaten, these selectivity values provide little new information. Among the remaining groups, only nematocerous Diptera had large enough



================================================================================

130 Psyche [VOI. 90
Table 2.
Spider selectivity (Es) values for prey types collected from webs of A. tuonabo.
Prey type
- -
Collected from webs
Collected from webs
(eaten only) (eaten and uneaten)
no. å´" no.
Pi ES
Beetles 52 18.8 56 12.5 +0.20
Nematocerous Diptera
32 11.6
164 36.8 -0.52
Non-nematocerous Diptera
2 8 10. I
34 7.6 +0.14
Ants 1 16 42.0 128 28.7 +O. 19
Parasitoid Hymenoptera 32 11.6 42 9.4 +0.10 Others
16* 5.8
22** 4.8 -----
*Others include: butterflies (6), bees and wasps ( 10) **Others include: butterflies (6), bees and wasps (10, leafhoppers (4), thrips (2) numbers of eaten (32) and uneaten (1 32) individuals to permit com- parison. Mean body lengths for eaten (x = 1.6 mm; SD = 1.8) and uneaten (x = 0.8 mm; SD = 0.29) nematocerans were significantly different (t = 4.86; p <.001).
The present findings highlight 2 features of the predatory behav- ior of A. tuonabo. First, the webs captured and the spiders con- sumed nonrandom samples of the available prey. Nonrandom web captures have been recorded for other spiders (e.g. Uetz and Biere 1980; Brown 198 1; Turnbull 1960) and most likely reflect differing abilities for web avoidance or escape among different prey. While no avoidance was observed, I did see several large flies (Asilidae and Tabanidae) strike webs but then successfully escape. Among insects successfully restrained by the web, the spider may attack, ignore, or reject different types and/or sizes of prey. Numerous studies (e.g. Robinson and Robinson 1970, 1973; Riechert and Tracy 1975; Turnbull 1960) note rejected prey, but few studies (Uetz and Biere 1980) quantify attack vs. ignore probabilities for different prey. Here, the tendency of A. tuonabo to ignore nematocerans probably does not reflect avoidance but rather the inability of these small, weak-flying insects to escape or damage the web. Thus, A. tuonabo may have ignored these weak prey only to consume them with their web in the evening. Interestingly, the mean body length of nema- tocerans being consumed was nearly twice that of nematocerans



================================================================================

Table 3.
Web selectivity (Ew values for size classes of nematocerous Diptera and ants collected from webs of A. iiionuho. a. Nematocerous Diptera
Collected from webs Captured on traps
Size (mm) (eaten and uneaten)
no. no. pi E w
b. Ants
Collected from webs Captured on traps
Size (mm) (eaten and uneaten)
no.
r i
no. pi
E w
0- 1 0 0.0 0 0.0
-----
-2 17
13.3 43 36.7 -0.47
2-3 I 1
8.6 46 39.3 -0.64
3-4 2 1 16.4 7 6.0 +0.46
4-5 16
12.5 3 2.6 +0.65
5-6 10
7.8 3 2.6 +0.50
6-7 24
18.7 7 6.0 +0.5 1
7-8 13
10.2 2 1.7 +0.7 1
>8 16
12.5 6 5.1 +0.42
caught in the web but ignored. Spider selectivity for larger prey has also recently been demonstrated for Micrathena gracilis (Uetz and Biere 1980).
Second, A. tuonabo did not construct their webs at heights where total prey abundance was greatest. Since the taxonomic and size composition of the flying insect fauna varied only slightly with height, A. tuonabo was apparently not responding to the vertical distribution of a particular type or size of prey. Several factors potentially affect web height in A. tuonabo. First, although females use various support structures, the number of suitable "web spaces" may be limited (Lubin pers. comm.). Also, other species of similar size (e .g. Pronous tuberculifer, Edricus crassicaudus, and Leucauge sp.) construct webs closer to the ground (Lubin 1978; Shelly pers. obs.). Thus, the higher webs of A. tuonabo may reflect a behavioral means to lessen interspecific competition for food. In addition,



================================================================================

132 Psyche IVol. 90
increased web height may reduce risks of predation from ground- or litter-dwelling predators.
1 thank H. Levi, M. Robinson, Y. Lubin, and particularly C. Olive for helpful comments on an earlier draft. H. Levi identified the species. D. Weinberger kindly helped process the sticky trap samples. The Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute provided logistic support.
BROWN. K.
1981.
Foraging ecology and niche partitioning in orb-weaving spiders. Oeco- logia 50: 380-385.
CIIAMHHRI,IN, R. V. AND W. 1i.11..
1936.
New spiders from Mexico and Panama. Bull. Univ. Utah 27: 1-103. CROAT, T. B.
1978.
Flora of Barro Colorado Island. Stanford Univ. Press. Stanford. HOI-DRIDGI~. L. R.. W. C. GRI~NKI,. W. H. HAT~II~WAY. T. LIANG, ANDJ. A. TOSI, JR. 1971.
Forest environments in tropical life zones: a pilot study. Pergamon Press, San Francisco.
l~1.h~. V. S.
1961.
Experimental ecology of the feeding of fishes. Yale Univ. Press, New Haven.
LIKIN. Y. D.
1978.
Seasonal abundance and diversity of web-building spiders in relation to habitat structure on Barro Colorado Island. Panama. J. Arachnol. 6: 31-51.
01,1\T. c. w.
1980.
Foraging specialimtions in orb-weaving spiders. Ecology 61: 1 133-1 144. RIIXHI~RT. S. E. AND C. R. TRACY.
1975.
Thermal balance and prey availability: bases for a model relating web- site characteristics to spider reproductive success. Ecology 56: 265-284. RII~CIII~RT, S. E. AND J. L~CZAK.
1982. Spider foraging: behavioral responses to prey. In Spider communica- tion. eds. P. N. Witt and J. S. Rovner. Princeton Univ. Press. Princeton, NJ.
ROBINSON. M. H. AND B. ROBINSON.
1970. Prey caught by a sample population of the spider Argiope argeniaia (Araneae: Araneidae) in Panama: a year's census data. Zool. J. Linn. soc. 49: 345-357.
R~HINSON. M. H. AND B. ROBINSON.
1973.
Ecology and behavior of the giant wood spider Nephila maculata (Fabricius) in New Guinea. Smithsonian Contrib. Zool. No. 149.



================================================================================

T~RNBI-LL, A. L.
1960. The prey of the spider Lin,~~phia rriangu1ari.s (Clerck) (Araneae, Linyphiidae). Can. J. Zool. 38: 859-873. UKTZ, G. W.. A. D. J~IINSON. AND D. W. SCHHMSKE. 1978.
Web placement, web structure, and prey capture in orb-weaving spiders. Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 4: 141-148.
UETZ, G. W. AND J. M. BIERH.
1980. Prey of Miu-uihenu gran'li.~ (Walckenaer) (Araneae: Araneidae) in com- parison with artificial webs and other trapping devices. Bull. Br. Arach- nol. SOC. 5: 101-107.




================================================================================




================================================================================


Volume 90 table of contents