Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
This is the CEC archive of Psyche through 2000. Psyche is now published by Hindawi Publishing.

Maria Guadalupe Del Rio Pesado and Thomas M. Alloway.
Polydomy in the slave-making ant, Harpagoxenus americanus (Emery) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae).
Psyche 90(1-2):151-162, 1983.

This article at Hindawi Publishing: https://doi.org/10.1155/1983/63051
CEC's scan of this article: http://psyche.entclub.org/pdf/90/90-151.pdf, 820K
This landing page: http://psyche.entclub.org/90/90-151.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted automatically from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

POLYDOMY IN THE SLAVE-MAKING ANT,
HA RPAGOXENUS A MERICANUS (EMERY)
(HYMENOPTERA: FORMICIDAE)1
MARIA GUADALUPE DEL RIO PESADO AND THOMAS M. ALLOWAY Erindale College
University of Toronto
Mississauga, Ontario L5L 1 C6
Canada
Slavery in ants is a form of social parasitism in which parasitic 'slave-making" species exploit the labor of workers derived from host-species colonies. The slave makers raid host-species nests, where they capture all or part of the brood. Subsequently, workers maturing from the captured brood form a social attachment to the slave makers and perform all the usual worker-ant functions in the parasites' colony (see review in Buschinger et a/. 1980). Harpagoxenus americanus (Emery) is an obligatory slave maker living in eastern North America, where it forms mixed colonies with members of certain Leptothorax species (see Alloway 1979). Two kinds of H. americanus nests are found: "primary nests" containing a single slave-maker queen and slaves with or without slave-maker workers, and "secondary nests" consisting of slave-maker workers and slaves without a slave-maker queen (Creighton 1927; Sturtevant 1927; Buschinger & Alloway 1977). Primary nests are apparently established when a parasite queen successfully invades a host- species nest (Wesson 1939). but the origin of secondary nests is questionable. The problem is compounded by the fact that secon- dary nests are usually more numerous than primary nests and fre- quently produce slave-maker females (workers and/ or queens) from 'This research was supported by a scholarship from CONACYT [Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia. Mexico] to the first author and by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (Canada) to the second author. The authors would like to thank Victor Chudin for his assistance in collecting the data and Robin Stuart for his constructive comments on the manuscript. Manuscript received by the editor February 19. 1983.



================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 90
their broods (Wesson 1939; Buschinger & Alloway, 1977). In the related European slave maker, H. sublaevis, "ergatoid queens" (individuals with fully functional ovaries and a spermatheca, but with a more or less worker-like external morphology) are common and function as the usual female reproductives (Buschinger, 1978). However, Buschinger and Alloway (1977) found that, while many H. americanus workers have functional ovaries and lay eggs that can mature to produce males, they rarely, if ever, possess a spermat- eca. Thus, "ergatoid queens" are absent or very rare in H. arneri- canus. Moreover, these authors thought that thelytoky was unlikely in this species.
Wesson (1939) observed the formation of secondary nests in the laboratory. Slave-maker colonies which had conducted several ordinary slave raids sometimes concluded the final raid of the sea- son by splitting into two components. In these cases, a few slave- maker workers and slaves remained indefinitely in the raided nest with part of the captured brood. Wesson suggested that this might be a sufficiently common late-season activity to account for the frequent occurrence of secondary nests. However, even if secondary nests are formed in this manner, there are still two possibilities for the relationship between secondary and primary nests and for the origin of the female slave-maker brood in secondary nests. Follow- ing their formation, secondary nests might become autonomous entities functionally separate from their parental primary nests. In this case, if we exclude thelytoky, the female slave-maker brood in secondary nests would have to be derived exclusively from brood carried over from the primary nest when the secondary nest was initially occupied (Buschinger & Alloway 1977). Alternatively, the primary nest and one or more secondary nests might comprise a single multiple-nest (polydomus) colony (Sturtevant 1927). Interac- tions between the nests of such polydomous colonies would be pro- tracted, and the slave-maker queen would continue to supply female brood for all nests in her colony. The latter possibility is supported by the fact that polydomy of this type has recently been demon- strated in two of the host species of H. americanus, Leptothorax ambiguus Emery and L. longispinosus Roger (Alloway et al. 1982). The objective of the present study was to examine these two possibilities by collecting and mapping H. arnericanus and host- species nests in nature, reconstructing their spatial relationships in the laboratory, and observing the interactions among them.



================================================================================

19831 Pesado & A1lwa.1- - Harpagoxenus americanus 153 The ants were collected on the Erindale Campus of the University of Toronto in Mississauga, Ontario, during the spring and summer of 1980 and 1981. Since our purpose was to determine whether colonies of H. americanus sometimes occupy more than one nest, we looked for areas where two H. americanus nests occurred within 2 m of each other. Whenever such a place was located, we layed out a 2 m by 2 m quadrant centering on the two nests and then collected, numbered and mapped the location of every H. americanus nest and every nest of its host species (L. ambiguus and L. longispinosus) in the quadrant. In some cases, adjacent quadrants were combined to permit the collection of a larger group of slave-maker nests. In the laboratory, we removed the ants and their brood from their natural nests and established them in artificial nests of the type described by Alloway (1979). For censusing, the artificial nests were placed in petri dishes (diameter = 14.5 cm; height = 1.5 cm) contain- ing a water bottle and food (Bhatkar & Whitcomb 1970). Then the ants were transported to an unairconditioned, naturally lighted room. On the floor of this room, quadrants were layed out with masking tape; and the field maps were used to locate the position occupied by each nest. A thick layer of petroleum jelly on the mask- ing tape formed a barrier which confined the ants to their respective quadrants. A water bottle and food were placed near each nest. In this way, it was possible to set up the artificial nests so that we duplicated the spatial arrangement of the natural nests. In addition to the quadrants collected from the field, we set up one control quadrant to study behavioral interactions between two H. americanus nests which had not been collected near one another in nature. The sides of this control quadrant were 100 cm long, and the two nests were placed 80 cm apart.
During the course of our observations, some of the ants were marked so that they could be individually identified. Each mark consisted of a very small dot of colored nail polish applied to the dorsal surface of the gaster with the tip of a minuten pin embedded in the end of a wooden stick. Ants remained marked for periods of 1 day to 1 month.
Observations were made 8 h a day, 5 days a week between 10 June and 27 August 1980 and between 7 May and 30 August 198 1. Five quadrants were collected and observed during 1980; and 14 quad- rants were collected and observed during 198 1.



================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 90
There was a total of 19 quadrants. However, quadrant 1 was merged with quadrant 2 and quadrant 9 with quadrant 10 when an additional H. americanus nest was found in close proximity to a group of other H. americanus nests, but outside the original quad- rant boundary. Altogether, the quadrants contained 49 H. ameri- canus nests, 57 L. ambiguus nests, and 59 L. longispinosus nests (see Table 1).
Our most common observation was "fusion" of all the H. ameri- canus nests in a quadrant. By "fusion", we mean that eventually all the ants from two or more H. americanus nests peacefully moved into a single nest after exchanging adult nest-mates and brood among the different nests for varying lengths of time (Figure 1). This exchange was carried out exclusively by slaves. The ability of nests to fuse shows that there is no behavioral barrier to interactions and exchange of nestmates among nests and thus indicates either that all the ants are members of the same polydomous colony or that H. americanus is a unicolonial species with no behavioral barriers between its nests. Fusion of all the slave-maker nests was observed in quadrants (1 + 2), 5, 6, (9 + lo), 11, 12, 13, 16, 17, and 18, in which there was never more than one slave-maker queen. However, we did not always observe fusion among H. americanus nests:
a. In the control quadrant where two H. americanus nests from different collection sites were arbitrarily set up near each other, the ants showed no tendency to fuse; and the slave makers from one nest successfully raided the other slave-maker nest. b. Quadrants 3 and 8 each contained two H. americanus queens living in different nests with slaves and a brood. In quad- rant 3, there was little behavioral interaction between the ants in the two nests. However, in quadrant 8, after the brood matured, the H. americanus workers in one nest raided the other slave- maker nest.
c. In quadrant 4, there was little contact between the ants in the two H. americanus nests, but the small amount of contact observed was hostile.
d. In quadrant 14, slaves and slave makers in two nests fused and then fought with the slaves and slave makers in a third nest.



================================================================================

Figure 1 - Set-up date, duration of nestmate exchange until date of fusion.
NO. OF
QUAD.
I
1 set-up date
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
nestmate exchange until fusion.
14
15
16
17
18
19
10 20
30 10 20
30 10 20 30 10 20 30
MAY
JUNE
JULY AUGUST
\
I
\
i,
t
1
I
1




================================================================================

Psyche [VOI. 90
e. In quadrant 19, both the slaves and the slave makers from different nests fought whenever they met; and the slave makers in one nest mounted an incomplete raid against the other slave- maker nest.
These observations indicate that H. arnericanus is not a unicolo- nial species. Aggressive behavioral barriers preventing the exchange of nestmates and nest fusion exist between some H. americanus nests. This fact strengthens the conclusion that exchange of nest- mates and brood and nest fusion, when they do occur, are indicative of the existence of a polydomous colony. Somewhat peculiar partial fusions were observed in quadrants 7 and 15. In both quadrants, the slaves peacefully moved into a single nest. In quadrant 7, the slave makers from the two nests fought. In quadrant 15 where there were 5 H. americanus nests, some of the slaves attacked slave makers which had been living in other nests prior to the fusion. These partial fusions may represent situations in which lack of contact between nests had begun to produce auton- omy between nests.
Once the raiding season was over, we observed the formation of secondary nests in quadrants 1 and 6. In both quadrants, some of the ants which had been occupying a single nest moved into a second nest. In both cases, exchange of nestmates and brood con- tinued for two weeks, when observations ended. In 7 quadrants, we were unable to find an H. americanus queen, despite our efforts to collect each nest completely and to search beyond quadrant boundaries for additional slave-maker nests belong- ing to these nest groups (see Table 1). However, in each of these cases, all the maturing H. americanus adults were males, a fact which indicates that these particular nests had not been receiving female brood from a primary nest and is consistent with the suppo- sition (Buschinger & Alloway 1977) that thelytoky does not occur in H. americanus.
The total number of adults of various species in all nests studied is summarized in Table 1. The total number of H. americanus workers was 1 15, with the average slave-maker nest containing about 2 H. americanus workers. The largest number of slave makers in a single nest at the time of the original census was 13; and the largest number of nests in a single apparently polydomus H. americanus colony was 6 in quadrant (9 + 10). Altogether, this colony contained 19 H.



================================================================================

19831 Pesado & Allo~t,a.r - Harpagoxenus arnericanus 1 57 americanus workers, 54 L. longispinosus slaves and 53 L. ambiguus slaves. The average distance between H. americanus nests in nest groups apparently comprising a single colony was 43.5 cm, with a range of 1 1 to 159 cm. The average distance between H. americanus nests among which there were aggressive interactions was 61.36 cm, with a range of 19 to 180 cm.
In the H. americanus nests, L. longispinosus slaves outnumbered L. ambiguus slaves by a ratio of almost 4:1, the total number of slaves being 803 (79.5%) for L. longispinosus and 207 (20.5%) for L. ambiguus. All the H. americanus colonies contained L. longispino- sus slaves, and 9 colonies contained slaves of both species. However, none of the H. americanus colonies used in this study had only L. ambiguus slaves, although such colonies are occasionally found in the Toronto region (Alloway unpublished data). Nevertheless, 7 of the quadrants studied contained no nests of free-living L. longispino- sus, and one of the quadrants in which there were L. ambiguus slaves contained no nests of free-living L. ambiguus (see Table 1). Unenslaved nests of L. longispinosus were on average somewhat more populous than the unenslaved nests of L. ambiguus, the mean number of workers per nest being 25.6 for L. longispinosus and 16.8 for L. ambiguus.
Our observations indicate that many Harpagoxenus americanus colonies are polydomus. This conclusion is based primarily on observations of peaceful interactions and of nest fusion among nests collected close together in nature, contrasted with observations that ants from different H. americanus nests do not always interact peacefully. The fighting and raiding observed indicate that H. ameri- canus does not possess a unicolonial population structure. Thus, peaceful exchange of nestmates and nest fusions, when they occur, signify the existence of polydomous colonies. However, polydomy in H. americanus is not obligatory. New colonies are monodomous, becoming polydomous as they grow. Finally, our observations of partial fusions suggest that nests in polydomus colonies may gradu- ally become autonomous, perhaps due to cessation of regular con- tact between nests. Under these circumstances, new queenless "secondary colonies," similar to those envisaged by Wesson ( 1939). could be formed.




================================================================================

Psyche
IVol. 90




================================================================================

H a = Harpagoxenus americanus.
L a = Leptothorax ambiguus.
L 1 = Leptothorax longispinosus.
Q = queen. $ = worker.




================================================================================

160 Psyche [VOI. 90
Nevertheless, the fact that ants which had been living in several different nests in nature so frequently moved into a single artificial nest in the laboratory is somewhat problematic. Under our labora- tory conditions, polydomy seldom persisted, possibly because our artificial nests were somewhat more spacious than the acorn nests which these ants inhabit in nature. If the ants live in more than one acorn because no single acorn is large enough for the whole colony, then giving the colony a larger artificial nest might produce nest fusion. However, many factors other than space may be involved in producing and maintaining polydomy in these ants (see discussion in Del Rio Pesado, 1983).
Our observations of nest divisions in two quadrants further sup- ports the polydomy hypothesis. However, the nest fusions which we saw did not closely resemble those described by Wesson (1939). In only one of our colonies did raiding parties tend to remain in target nests; and even these raiders returned home after 1 to 3 days. What we observed was that ants which had been occupying one nest came to occupy two nests after the "raiding season" was over. Several previous investigators have noted that many H. ameri- canus nests are queenless (Buschinger & Alloway 1977; Creighton 1927; Sturtevant 1927; Wesson 1939). The usual conjecture has been that most of these queenless nests are "branches" located near queenright nests. Our data confirm this supposition by showing that many queenless nests are parts of queenright polydomus colonies. However, there were 7 quadrants in which we could not find a nest containing an H. americanus queen. Since these queenless nests produced only male slave-maker brood, it is unlikely that they represent components of a queen-right polydomous colony; and the males produced in these nests are probably the offspring of H. americanus workers (Buschinger and Alloway 1977). Some of these isolated nests may be remnants of colonies whose queen has died, while others may be products of long-distance raids from which the raiders failed to return. The presence in some H. americanus nests of slaves belonging to a species for which there were no free-living nests in the same quadrant suggests that H. americanus raids may occur over distances of several meters; and far-ranging raiders may sometimes fail to return to their base (Creighton 1927). In our study area, L. longispinosus slaves outnumbered L. ambi- guus slaves by a ratio of almost 4: 1. This finding is typical through- out southern Ontario and the adjacent parts of New York state,



================================================================================

19831 Pesado & Allow~ay - Harpagoxenus americanus 16 1 despite the fact that L. ambiguus colonies are generally more abundant than L. longispinosus colonies (Alloway et al. 1982). Two factors probably account for the prevalence of L. longispinosus slaves in H. americanus nests. First, H. americanus seems to mani- fest an ecological preference for rather cool, shady places, a habitat preference which closely matches that of L. longispinosus. Second, at our study site, we found that L. longispinosus nests were on average ore populous than L. ambiguus nests. Thus, a raid against a nest of L. longispinosus might net more worker pupae than a raid against a nest of L. ambiguus.
Field maps were made while collecting nests of the slave-making ant, Harpagoxenus americanus, and two of its host species, Lepto- thorax ambiguus and L. longispinosus. The ants were then trans- ferred to artificial nests arranged to reconstruct the natural spatial relationships among nests. Ants from adjacent slave-maker nests often exchanged nestmates and brood for a period of time before moving into a single nest; and ants which had been living in a single nest in the laboratory sometimes moved into two nests. However, in other instances, ants from adjacent nests fought. These observations were interpreted as indicating that colonies of H. americanus some- times occupy more than one nest (facultative polydomy). Nest popu- lation data were also presented and discussed. ALLOWAY, T. M.
1979.
Raiding behaviour of two species of slave-making ants, Harpagoxenus americanus (Emery) and Leptoihorax duloticus Wesson. Animal Behav- iour 27: 202-2 10.
ALLOWAY. T. M.. A. BUSCHINGER, M. TALBOT. R. STUART. & C. THOMAS. 1982.
Polygyny and polydomy in three North American species of the ant genus Leptothorax Mayer (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Psyche, 89: 249-290.
BHATKAR, A. & W. H. WHITCOMB.
1970.
Artificial diet for rearing species of ants. Fla. Entomologist 53: 217-232. BUSCHINGER, A.
1978.
Genetically induced origin of alate females in the slave-making ant, Har- pagoxenus sublaevis (Nyl.) (Hym., form). Insectes Sociaux 25: 163-172. BUSCHINGER, A., & T. M. ALLOWAY.
1977.
Population structure and polymorphism in the slave-making ant Harpa- goxenus americanus (Emery). Psyche 83: 233-242.



================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 90
BUSCHINGER, A., W. EHRHARDT, AND U. WINTER. 1980.
The organization of slave-raids in dulotic ants: A comparative study (Hymenoptera: Formicidea). Zeitschriftfur Tierpsychologie 53: 245-264. CREIGHTON, w. s.
1927. The slave raids of Harpagoxenus americanus. Psyche, 36: 48-80. DEL RIO PESADO, M. G.
1983. Polydomy in the slave-making ant Harpagoxenus americanus (Emery) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). M.Sc. Thesis, University of Toronto. STURTEVANT, A. H.
1927.
The social parasitism of the ant Harpagoxenus americanus. Psyche 34: 1-9.
WESSON, L. G., JR.
1939.
Contribution to the natural history of Harpagoxenus americanus Emery (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Transactions of the American Entomolog- ical Society 65: 97- 122.
WHEELER, W. M.
1910. Ants: Their Structure, Development, and Behavior. Columbia Univer- sity Press, N.Y. & London, 663 pp.
WILSON, E. 0.
1971. The Insect Societies. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cam- bridge, 548 pp.




================================================================================


Volume 90 table of contents