Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
This is the CEC archive of Psyche through 2000. Psyche is now published by Hindawi Publishing.

Robin J. Stuart.
Spontaneous polydomy in laboratory colonies of the ant, Leptothorax curvispinosus Mayr (Hymenoptera: Formicidae).
Psyche 92(1):71-81, 1985.

This article at Hindawi Publishing: https://doi.org/10.1155/1985/29215
CEC's scan of this article: http://psyche.entclub.org/pdf/92/92-071.pdf, 1100K
This landing page: http://psyche.entclub.org/92/92-071.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted automatically from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

SPONTANEOUS POLYDOMY IN LABORATORY COLONIES OF THE ANT LEPTOTHORAX CURV/SP/NOSUS MAYR (HYMENOPTERA; FORMICIDAE)
BY ROBIN J. STUART*
Department of Zoology, Erindale College, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario Canada L5L 1 C6
Lqtothorax curvispinosus Mayr is a small, common ant in east- ern North America where it often nests in such preformed cavities as hollow acorns, hickory nuts, galls, stems and twigs. Its range and nesting preferences overlap broadly with those of two closely- related species, L. ambiguus and L. longispinosus (Creighton 1950; Alloway et al. 1982). Colonies within this group are both intra- and interspecifically aggressive; and although normally considered nonparasitic, they occasionally engage in facultative interspecific slavery, apparently by capturing and rearing brood from one anoth- er's colonies. Facultative intraspecific slavery may occur more fre- quently (Alloway 1980). In addition, these species serve as hosts for the obligatory slave-makers Harpagoxenus americanus and L. dulo- ticus (Alloway 1979; for terminology see Stuart and Alloway 1983). All five species coexist in some areas, nest in close proximity to one another, and form dense local populations (Alloway et al. 1982; Del Rio Pesado and Alloway 1983).
Approximately one third (36.3%) of individual L. curvispinosus nests (e.g. single acorns) are queenless, while about half (46.7%) are monogynous, and the remainder (17.0%) are polygynous (Headley 1943; Talbot 1957; Alloway et al. 1982). Dissections have shown that polygyny in this species is more than just a superficial phenom- enon with over 90% of the queens in polygynous nests being inseminated and reproductive. Moreover, young queens often coex- ist with older queens, indicating that established colonies may *Present address: Museum of Comparative Zoology Laboratories. Harvard Univer- sity, Cambridge, Mass. 02138.
Manuscript received b.~, the editor September 19, 1984 Ps~che 92:7 1-82 (1985). hup Wpsycht einclub org/dMMm.htinl



================================================================================

IL rsycne "VOI. YZ
become secondarily polygynous by accepting young queens in* o their nests (Alloway et al. 1982; for a review of polygyny in ants see Holldobler and Wilson 1977). The large proportion of queenless nests led Sturtevant (1927) and Talbot (1957) to suggest that colo- nies may occupy multiple nests (polydomy); and Sturtevant < 1927) provided some experimental evidence for this hypothesis by noting that nests found relatively close together in nature were occasi. onally non-aggressive toward one another. Similar evidence indicator's that colonies of L. ambiguus, L. longispinosus and H. americanus c a n be polydomous as well (Alloway et al. 1982; Del Rio Pesado a n d Allo- way 1983).
The discovery of polygyny and polydomy in this group of ants raises numerous questions concerning the structure of colonies and local populations. Colonies possessing these characteristics m a y be capable of existing indefinitely, and of spreading out and occupying large areas of suitable habitat. Such "super colonies" are liighly developed in other groups of ants with these characteristics (e.g. Formica yessensis Forel, Higashi and Yamuchi 1979). In add ition, there are many questions regarding factors which induce polydomy (both proximately and ultimately) and the social ramifications of this colony structure (see Werner et al. 1979). Leptothoracin e ants are especially amenable to laboratory culture and provide a n unu- sual opportunity for the experimental study of polydomy. I n the present experiment, L. curvispinosus colonies (or colony fragments) were collected from individual nests in nature and provided with multiple artificial nests in the laboratory for an extended period of time. The purpose of the experiment was to discover whether colo- nies would become polydomous (i.e. "bud", see below) spontane- ously under relatively simple laboratory conditions. Traditionally, the term "budding" has been used to refer t. o true colony fission in which a group of workers and one or more repro- ductive~ depart from the parental nest to form an autonornous daughter colony (see Wilson 197 1). However, for polydomou s spe- cies, the term "budding" has also been used to describe the forma- tion of non-autonomous secondary nests which at least initial1 y may be queenless (e.g. Higashi 1976). In general, the relative auto nomy of individual nests as reproductive units within polydomous colo- nies is debatable, and it is possible that such nests occasionally become isolated and achieve full autonomy (see Alloway e t al.



================================================================================

19851 ~tuart - uptoinorax cmspznosus 1-5 1982). For these reasons, and for convenience, I suggest the con- tinued use of the term "budding" to refer to the formation of daugh- ter nests which contain brood and are spatially distinct from the parental nest, but which may or may not contain reproductives, and which may or may not become truly autonomous. The 57 colonies in this experiment were collected in early J u n e from acorns and hickory nuts at several sites in the vicinity of H r-
persfield, Ohio. The inhabitants of each nest were treated as sepa- rate colonies and established in separate artificial nests and foraging dishes in the laboratory (see Alloway 1979). The artificial nests we re composed of two parts. The nest base consisted of the top of a small plastic petri dish (dia = 4.0 cm; ht = 0.5 cm) painted green and drilled with a small hole (dia = 0.3 cm) in one side to serve as a nest entrance. The nest lid was made of transparent orange Plexiglas, milled with a groove so as to fit tightly onto the nest base. With tile lid in place, the internal volume of these nests was approximate1 y 4 cm3 (k 0.2). Each artificial nest was housed in a foraging dish co n- sisting of a large plastic petri dish (dia = 14.5 cm; ht = 1.5 cm). Each foraging dish also contained a water bottle (dia = 1.1 cm; length = 3.5 cm) stoppered with cotton and held in place at one side of the dish by a dab of plasticene. A microscope slide (2.5 X 7.5 cm) served as a food platter in each dish. Water bottles were refilled weekly, and fresh food was added to the food platter 3 times p e r week. The food was similar to that formulated by Bhatkar and Whitcomb (1970), but was prepared in bulk, frozen, and then thawed prior to use. Food platters were replaced periodically. The colonies were cultured on open laboratory benches illuminated toy fluorescent lights mounted approximately 50 cm above the bench surface and providing an illumination of 1830-2 150 lux. A da i ly photoperiod of l4L: 10D, and a temperature of 22OC ? lo were maintained.
The experiment was initiated in early July, by adding a second artificial nest to each foraging dish, such that the two nest entrances were facing each other, approximately 4-5 cm apart. Each colony was briefly observed for budding on days 1, 2, 5, 1 1, 13, 18, 29 and 76 following initiation. A colony was considered to be polydo- mous if brood was present in both artificial nests when observed.



================================================================================

14 rsycne [VOI. 92
Table 1.
Census of adults in each nest at the beginning of the experiment: Qd = dealate queens, Qa = alate queens, 8 = males, $ = workers (k 2). The figure in brackets after the Qd count indicates the number of dealate queens that were present in the nest at the time of collection. Asterisks (*) indicate colonies which budded during the experiment (see Figure I).
# Qd Qa 8 $ # Qd Qa 8 $ # $d Qa 8 $
I*
2(1)
2 0 150
20
l(1)
3 0 55 39
l(1) 0 3 40
2
l(1) 0 0
45
21
3(0) 4 4
35
40
O(0) 0 18 15
3
o(o)
0 7 10
22
l(1) 0 0
45
41
2(1) 0 0130
4
11(1) 6
0155
23 l(1) 0 0
20
42
9(1)
4
28 20
5*
18(1)
8
7 60
24 l(1) 1 0
45
43
l(1) 0
0
35
6
3 (3)
0
0 85 25 2 (1)
0 0
90
44
3 (0) 24
1
20
7
O(0)
0 0 70
26 l(1)
0 0 50
45
2(1)
0
4 90
8*
O(0) 0
5 30 27 2(2)
0 0
25
46
3(1) 7
0
90
9
6(1)
0
0 75
28 I(I)
0 0
50
47
l(1) 0
0
30
10
1(1) 0
0 45 29 4(1)
0 0
60
48
15(1) 4
1
90
11*
2(1)
5
0 75
30 I(!) 0 0
50
49
12(1) 4
0
55
12
O(0) 8
0 50
31 l(1)
0 0 70
50*
2(1) 3
1
75
13
l(1) 0
0 105
32 l(1)
0 0 95
51
3(1)
12 0
30
14*
1(1) 0
0 105
33 l(1) 0 0
45
52*
7(2)
13 2
90
15-
6(2) 18
0160
34 l(1)
0 0 80
53
3(0) 3
1 10
16
O(0) 2
11 40
35 l(1)
0 0 60
54
O(0) 6
2 35
17*
3(2) 0
0 110
36 l(0)
5 22
15
55
l(0) 8
0
15
18*
l(2) 1
0 130
37 8(0)
5 25 15
56
l(1)
4 5
55
l9*
2(1) 5
0 60
38 13 (0)
7 0 10
57*
14(1)
6
0 170
Colonies remained undisturbed except for these observations and the normal maintenance indicated above. The data were analysed using Chi-square tests and Student's t-tests. Chi-square tests involv- ing one degree of freedom employed a correction for continuity, and all comparisons of census data with t-tests utilized a square root transformation (Parker 1979).
Census data for each colony are given in Table 1. Between the time the colonies were collected and the initiation of the experiment many of the larger larvae pupated and eclosed to produce workers, alate females and males. Many of the young queens subsequently became dealate, and at least some appeared to function as workers. Nonetheless, males and females were observed flying about in their foraging dishes, and some mating between siblings may have occurred, although none was observed. None of the queens was dissected in this study, and hence the reproductive status of both young and old queens remains unknown. Little colony growth occurred after the initiation of the experiment.



================================================================================

The queen status of colonies at the beginning of the experiment often differed from their queen status at the time of collection due to the occasional death of queens or the dealation of offspring (see Table 1). At the beginning of the experiment, there was no signifi- cant difference in the number of workers present in colonies which were queenless, monogynous or polygynous at that time (t-tests, P > 0.05). However, worker numbers at the beginning of the exper- iment did vary depending on what the queen status of colonies had been at the time of collection, with originally queenless colonies having significantly fewer workers than colonies which had been either monogynous (t = 4.241, df = 49, P < 0.001) or polygynous (t = 3.347, df = 18, P < 0.0 1) when collected. This variation will be considered in the analysis of the results. During the course of the experiment, 12 of the 57 colonies (2 1.1 %) became polydomous (see Table 1 and Figure 1). Six colonies were observed to be polydomous on Day 1 (10.5%); and 2 more became polydomous by Day 2 (14.0%). Thereafter, the number of polydo- mous colonies fluctuated between 2 and 7 (3.5-12.3%). Some colo- nies appeared to remain polydomous only briefly (e.g. #8, #19), but others apparently persisted in this condition for long periods (e.g. # 1, # 15, # 17, #52, #57) (see Figure 1). One colony (#52) appeared to remain polydomous for the entire experiment (76 days). Two colo- nies became polydomous, reverted to monodomy, and then again became polydomous (#14, #15). Other colonies may also have undergone repeated fissions and fusions between observations. There was no discernible pattern to how polydomous colonies distributed themselves between the two nests. Often only a few workers and larvae were present in the second nest, but at times large segments of the colonies including dealate queens and alate reproductives could be found in the second nest. The distribution for particular colonies tended to fluctuate between observations, and often the second nest (or the original nest) gradually became depopulated over several days prior to fusion. These kinds of fluc- tuations indicate that polydomy in these ants is a very dynamic phenomenon and provide considerable scope for future research. Budding occurred in queenless, monogynous and polygynous colonies containing from 30 to 170 workers (see Table l), but colony



================================================================================

Psyche [VOI. 92
Day of Experiment
-
Tot a
Days
25
1
1
2
9
69
29
2
1
12
76
29
Fig. 1.
Occurrence and apparent duration of spontaneous polydomy for all colo- nies which budded during the course of the experiment. Indicated are colony numbers (Col #). day of experiment on which polydomy was observed (Day of Experiment), number of colonies which were polydomous on each observation day (n/Day), and total number of days over which each colony appeared to remain polydomous (Total Days).
size (as indicated by the number of workers) appeared to have a significant influence on polydomy (see Figure 2). Budding occurred in 11 of 26 colonies (42.3%) which contained 60 or more workers, but in only 1 of 31 colonies (3.2%) with less than 60 workers (x * = 9.40, df = 1, P < 0.01). Overall, colonies which budded were sign ifi- cantly larger than those which failed to do so (t = 3.554, df = 55, P < 0.001). This relationship held even if colonies which were queenless when collected (n = 14) were omitted from the analysis (t = 3.268, df = 41, P< 0.01); or if colonies which were either queenless (n = 14) or polygynous (n = 6) when collected were omitted (t = 2.134, d f = 35, P < 0.05). Among colonies with 60 or more workers there was no significant difference in size between colonies which budded and those which did not (t = 1.305, df = 24, P > 0.05).



================================================================================

Stuart - Leptothorax curvispinosus
Workers
Fig. 2. The relationship between the occurrence of spontaneous polydomy and colony size, as indicated by plotting percent budding relative to the number of workers present. The number of colonies in each size category is indicated above each bar. The significance of this relationship is discussed in the text. It is possible that polygyny may influence the occurrence of polydomy. In this study only 6 colonies were polygynous when collected; and the 4 of these which budded were fairly large (see Table I). However, this sample is too small for meaningful statisti- cal comparisons. By the time the experiment was initiated, many colonies had become at least superficially polygynous due to the



================================================================================

7 8 Psyche [vo~. 92
dealation of offspring. However, colonies which were polygynous at this time (n = 27) did not bud more frequently than other colonies (x2 = 2.65, df = 1, P> 0.05); and, within this group, colonies which budded were significantly larger than those which failed to do so (t = 2.365, df = 25, P < 0.05).
The results of this study provide further empirical evidence for the existence of polydomy in L. curvispinosus colonies (see Sturtevant 1927). Moreover, these results demonstrate that at least some of the factors which are capable of inducing polydomy in these ants remain active under relatively simple laboratory conditions. The observed relationship between colony size and the occurrence of polydomy suggests that space limitations within nests could be a factor promoting polydomy. Nesting sites such as hollow nuts and twigs might well impose this kind of limitation on colonies in nature. However, other factors associated with colony size, age or maturity could also be involved. Furthermore, it is likely that var- ious factors can effectively induce polydomy in species in which colonies are capable of existing in this condition. In nature, colonies might become polydomous in response to the distribution of food around their nests or to exploit different humidities or temperatures in different nests, perhaps selectively distributing their brood or reproductives in this manner. The present experiment, with its uni- form plastic nests and simplistic foraging environment, provided little opportunity for these kinds of factors to operate. Among leptothoracine ants, a polydomous colony structure could have various important ramifications, depending at least in part on the relative degree of contact between nests within colonies. This colony structure might affect the way in which reproductive rights are apportioned among different queens and workers (see Wilson 1974a, b; Cole 198 1 ; Franks and Scovell l983), and it might influence the relative susceptibility of colonies to social parasites (see Alloway et al. 1982; Del Rio Pesado and Alloway 1983).
Indeed, the combination of polygyny and polydomy is thought to have been an important factor in the evolution of various forms of social parasitism (Buschinger 1970; Alloway 1980). The existence of polydomy in these ants may explain a peculiar element of their nest emigration behaviour. Moglich (1978) investi-



================================================================================

19851 Stuart - Leptothorax curvispinosus 79 gated this behaviour in various Leptothorax species, including L. curvispinosus and L. longispinosus, and found that these ants typi- cally use both tandem running and adult transport as recruitment techniques during emigrations. Tandem running is a chemical recruitment technique in which one ant leads a single follower between two points. The response is mediated by a pheromone orig- inating in the leader's poison gland and extruded from its sting (see Moglich et al. 1974; Moglich 1979; Stuart and Alloway 1983). Workers which are led to a new nest in this manner subsequently become recruiters themselves and lead or carry other ants to the new nest. However, most colony members, including workers, are merely carried to the new nest, and do not otherwise become involved in the emigration. The unusual element in this nest emigra- tion system is that inmany Leptothorax species tandem runs also occur in the reverse direction, from the new nest back to the old nest. Moglich could not rationalize the existence of these "reverse tandem runs" and, using colonies that were emigrating from a sin- gle occupied nest to an unoccupied nest, such recruitment appeared to contribute nothing to the efficiency of the emigration. However, if these species are frequently polydomous, emigrations may most commonly occur from one occupied nest to a second occupied nest, and reverse tandem runs could serve to recruit workers from the second nest to assist in the emigration. Thus, the discovery of polydomy may explain the existence of reverse tandem runs; and since this behaviour appears to be so widespread among leptothor- ache ants, it is possible that polydomy is also more common than we yet realize.
Leptothorax curvispinosus colonies derived from single nests in nature will frequently occupy multiple nests under simplified labor- atory conditions. Spontaneous polydomy of this type is apparently related to colony size. When budded, the relative distribution of a colony between alternative nests tends to fluctuate, and repeated fissions and fusions occur. These results provide further experimen- tal evidence for polydomy in this species.



================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 92
I thank T. M. Alloway, J. Carey and P. Boase for their helpful comments and criticisms. Financial assistance was provided by an Ontario Graduate Scholarship to the author and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant to T. M. Alloway.
AI.I.~WAV, T. M.
1979. Raiding behaviour of two species of slave-making ants, Hurpugoxenu~ ut~ierirunus ( Emery) and Leplo~horux duloticus Wesson (Hymen- optera: Formicidae). Anim. Behav. 27: 202-210. AI.I.~WAI, T. M. 1980.
The origins of slavery in leptothoracine ants (Hymen- optera: Formicidae). Amer. Nat. 115: 247 261. AI.I.~WAS. T. M.. A. Busciii~~ii:~. M. TAI.BOT, R. STUART AND C. THOMAS. 1982.
Polygyny and polydomy in three North American species of the ant genus I-eptothom Mayr (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Psyche 89: 249-274. BHATKAR, A,, AND W. H. WIIIIXWMH.
1970. Artificial diet for rearing various species of ants. Fla. Ent. 53: 21 7 232. BUS<'HINC~~~R. A. 1970. Neue Vorstellungen 7ur Evolution des So7ialparasitismus und der Dulosis bei Ameisen (Hymenoptera; Formicidae). Biol. Zentralbl. 88: 273 299. (English Translation)
Co1.1:. B. J. 1981. Dominance hierarchies in Leplolhorax ants. Science 212: 83-84.
CRI~IC~IIT~N, W. S. 1950.
Ants of North America. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool. 104: I 585.
Dhi. Rio PI.SADO. M. G., AND T. M. AILOWAY. 1983. Polydomy in the slave-
making ant, Hurpup.venu.s americunus (Emery) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Psyche 90: 15 1 - 162.
FRANKS, N. R., AND E. SCOVEI.I..
1983. Dominance and reproductive success among slave-making ants. Nature 304: 724-725. HIADI.I~S, A. E.
1943. Population studies of two species of ants, Leptolhorax lot~gi.v/~it?o.vu,v Roger and Leptolhorux vurvispinosus Mayr. Ann. Ent. Soc. Am. 36: 743- 753.
HIGASHI, S.
1976. Nest proliferation by budding and nest growth pattern in For- mica (Formica}re.v.w.si.s in Ishikari Shore. J. Fac. Sci. Hokkaido Univ. Ser. VI, ZOO]. 20: 359-389.
HIGASHI, S., AND K. YAMAIK'III. 1979. Influence of a supracolonial ant Formica (Formica) j-e.v.wmi.s Fore1 on the distribution of other ants in Ishikari Coast. Jap. J. Ecol. 29: 257-264.
HOi.i-~o~wi~, B., AND E. 0. WILSON.
1977. The number of queens: An important trait in ant evolution. Naturwissenschaften 64: 8-1 5. M~GI.K-H. M. 1978. Social organization of nest emigration in Leptuthorax (Hymenoptera. Formicidae). Ins. Soc. 25: 205-255.



================================================================================

Mttoi.icn, M. 1979. Tandem calling pheromone in the genus Leplothorax (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): Behavioral analysis of specificity. J. Chem. Ecol. 5: 35-52.
MOGI.K~, M., U. MASCIIWIT'/., AND B. HM,I.D~HI,HR. 1974. Tandem calling: a new kind of signal in ant communication. Science 186: 1046-1047. PARKI.R, R. E.
1979.
Introductory statistics for Biology. Arnold, London. v + 122 PP.
STUART. R. J., AND T. M. AI.I~WAY. 1983. The slave-making ant, Harpasoxenu.~ canadensis M. R, Smith, and its host-species, Leptothorax n~u.~(~oruni (Ny- lander): Slave raiding and territoriality. Behaviour 85: 58-90. STIIRTKVANT. A. H.
1927. The social parasitism of the ant Harpugoxenus anieri- canux. Psyche 34: 1-9.
TAI.IWT. M. 1957. Population studies of the slave-making ant Leptothora.~ dulo- ticu.~ and its slave Leptothorux curvispinosus. Ecology 38: 449-456. WERNER. P., F. CATZKI-1.1s AND D. Cnii~ix. 1979.
A propos du polycalisme che7
Formica (Co/>tofbrmira) exserta Nyl. CR Sect. Fr. UIEIS, Lausanne 7-8 Sept. 1979: 115- 126.
WII~N, E. 0.
1971. The insect societies. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass. x + 548 pp.
WII.SON. E. 0.
1974a. Aversive behavior and competition within colonies of the ant Lepiothoru.~ curvispinoxus. Ann. Ent. Soc. Am. 67: 777-780. WII.SON. E. 0.
1974b.
The population consequences of polygyny in the ant Lepio- thorax ctirviapinosus. Ann. Ent. Soc. Am. 67: 78 1-786.



================================================================================




================================================================================


Volume 92 table of contents